Enumeration skills in Down syndrome

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.07.038Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Visual enumeration skills were assessed in children with Down syndrome (DS).

  • Discrimination of small sets (subitizing) was impaired in DS relative to controls.

  • Discrimination of larger sets (number acuity) was in line with mental age.

  • Counting was less fluent but understanding of cardinality was preserved in DS.

  • The subitizing deficit suggests a specific impairment of the object tracking system.

Abstract

Individuals with Down syndrome (DS) exhibit various math difficulties which can be ascribed both to global intelligence level and/or to their atypical cognitive profile. In this light, it is crucial to investigate whether DS display deficits in basic numerical skills. In the present study, individuals with DS and two groups of typically developing (TD) children matched for mental and chronological age completed two delayed match-to-sample tasks in order to evaluate the functioning of visual enumeration skills. Children with DS showed a specific deficit in the discrimination of small numerosities (within the subitizing range) with respect to both mental and chronological age matched TD children. In contrast, the discrimination of larger numerosities, though lower than that of chronological age matched controls, was comparable to that of mental age matched controls. Finally, counting was less fluent but the understanding of cardinality seemed to be preserved in DS. These results suggest a deficit of the object tracking system underlying the parallel individuation of small numerosities and a typical – but developmentally delayed – acuity of the approximate number system for discrimination of larger numerosities.

Introduction

Down syndrome (DS) is due to abnormalities on chromosome 21 and it is the most common cause of intellectual disability (Kittler, Krinsky-McHale, & Devenny, 2008). The cognitive profile of this syndrome is characterized by a relative weakness in verbal abilities, while visuo-spatial skills seem to be relatively preserved (Dykens, Hodapp, & Finucane, 2000).

It is well known that children and adults with Down syndrome (DS) exhibit several mathematical difficulties as compared to typically developing (TD) individuals (Brigstocke, Hulme, & Nye, 2008). Children with DS obtain lower scores in a wide range of tests assessing basic math knowledge, arithmetic abilities and counting skills (Buckley and Sacks, 1987, Carr, 1988, Gelman and Cohen, 1988, Porter, 1999). These mathematical deficits can be attributed to the general intelligence level or to the atypical cognitive profile of DS. In this light, it is crucial to determine whether math underachievement in DS can be related to the low level of cognitive functioning or to specific deficits in basic numerical skills. Such an investigation may provide new insights regarding the source of difference in math achievement between DS and TD individuals.

Two basic pre-verbal mechanisms have been highlighted as fundamental for numerical processing in human and non-human species: the Object Tracking System (OTS; Mandler and Shebo, 1982, Trick and Pylyshyn, 1994, Xu et al., 2005) and the Approximate Number System (ANS; Dehaene, 1997, Feigenson et al., 2004, Piazza, 2010, Stoianov and Zorzi, 2012). The OTS is a domain-general system that encodes spatio-temporal characteristics of objects with a capacity limited to three-four items. Despite the fact that the OTS is primarily a non-numerical mechanism, it supports visual enumeration of small sets of objects. Indeed, observers can quickly, accurately and effortlessly perceive the numerosity of small sets, a phenomenon known as subitizing (from the Latin subitus, immediate; Kaufman, Lord, Reese, & Volkmann, 1949). Children with developmental dyscalculia have a reduced subitizing range and tend to adopt serial counting to determine the numerosity of small sets (Landerl et al., 2004, Moeller et al., 2009, Schleifer and Landerl, 2011), a finding that suggests a crucial role of the OTS mechanism for numerical development (Carey, 2001, Le Corre and Carey, 2007).

When the numerosity exceeds the subitizing range (i.e., more than 4 elements) and serial counting is precluded, visual enumeration become imprecise, with a variability of response that obeys Weber's law (Dehaene et al., 2008, Stoianov and Zorzi, 2012). This pattern, which is regarded as the signature of the ANS, implies that the ability to discriminate two numerosities decreases as a function of their numerical ratio and it can be indexed by the Weber fraction, also known as number acuity. This ability is already active during the first year of life (e.g., six months-old infants can discriminate 8 vs 16 dots; Xu & Spelke, 2000) and it is progressively refined during childhood (Halberda and Feigenson, 2008, Halberda et al., 2012, Piazza et al., 2010). Crucially, number acuity has been found to correlate with mathematical achievement (Halberda et al., 2008, Libertus et al., 2011, Lourenco et al., 2012, Mazzocco et al., 2011a) and it is severely reduced in children with developmental dyscalculia (Mazzocco et al., 2011b, Piazza et al., 2010).

The acquisition of a symbolic number system allows children to go beyond the pre-verbal number processing mechanisms. In particular, learning of the list of number words and the acquisition of a counting routine allows for accurate serial enumeration of potential infinite sets. Counting entails three basic principles (Gallistel and Gelman, 1992, Gelman and Gallistel, 1978): (i) a one-to-one correspondence between each object and the corresponding word in the counting list; (ii) a stable (and correct) order of the counting list; and (iii) identification of the last word in the counting list as the numerosity (cardinality) of the set. Proficient mastering of counting skills has an important influence on early math achievement (Jordan et al., 2007, Passolunghi et al., 2006).

The investigation of basic numerical skills in individuals with DS is relatively sparse, despite the fact that these abilities might be at the hearth of their math underachievement in comparison with typically developing children. The functioning of OTS and ANS was previously investigated by Paterson, Girelli, Butterworth, and Karmiloff-Smith (2006) in individuals with DS as compared to a group of individuals with William syndrome and to control groups matched for mental age and chronological age. Using a preferential looking paradigm, they found that children with DS (Mage = 30 months) did not discriminate between two and three objects, thereby suggesting a deficit in OTS. In contrast, performance of young adults with DS (Mage = 24 years-old) in a numerosity comparison task (using sets both within and beyond the subitizing range) was similar to that of control individuals. More recently, Camos (2009) reported that six year-old children with DS were able to discriminate between 16 and 8 dots but failed to discriminate between 12 and 8, thereby showing the classic ratio-dependent signature of ANS. Their performance was comparable to that of typically developing pupils (both MA and CA matched controls), but the limitation to two numerical ratios might have hidden potential differences in number acuity between children with DS and control groups. In summary, the ANS appears to be preserved in individuals with DS, whereas the OTS seems to be less efficient, at least in young children with DS.

A debated issue regarding counting skills in individuals with DS is whether they have a superficial or a deep understanding of counting (for a review, Abdelahmeed, 2007). On one hand, some studies suggest that individuals with DS use counting as a mere routine lacking the understanding of cardinality principle. Indeed, Gelman and Cohen (1988) maintained that children with DS learn to count by rote and often lack the knowledge of the cardinality principle. Porter (1999) also reported that children with DS can count by rote but are less efficient to detect counting errors performed by other individuals. On the other hand, different studies support the idea that individuals with DS properly understand the meaning of counting as well as the cardinality principle. For example, Caycho, Gunn, and Siegal (1991) found a similar understanding of counting principles in children with DS and control children matched for receptive vocabulary. Similarly, Bashash, Outhred, and Bochner (2003) found that children with DS were able to apply the three fundamental principles of counting in several counting contexts. Finally, Nye, Fluck, and Buckley (2001) reported a pattern of results in which children with DS demonstrated a conceptual understanding of cardinality, although they made more errors in the counting procedure. It clearly appears that the picture of the counting ability in DS is still controversial and it requires further investigation.

In the present study, we employed two numerosity match-to-sample tasks in order to evaluate the functioning of visual enumeration in children with DS in comparison to typically developing groups matched for both mental and chronological age. In both tasks, children observed a briefly presented sample numerosity and, after a delay period, a target numerosity. They had to decide whether the target numerosity was equal or different from the sample numerosity. In the dots-to-dots match-to-sample task, we assessed the ability to compare numerosities within and beyond subitizing range/OTS capacity. Our aim was to verify whether children with DS have a reduced subitizing range and to highlight possible differences in number acuity when numerosity discrimination entails larger numerosities. In the digit-to-dots match-to-sample task, the sample was an Arabic number and the target was a set of dots. Note that counting of the items in the target can yield the exact cardinality of the set to be matched with the cardinality entailed by the Arabic digit. Therefore, this task assessed the efficiency of the counting routine as well as the understanding of the cardinality principle

Section snippets

Participants

Sixty-three participants from the middle socioeconomic status from northern Italy took part to the study after parents gave their informed consent. There were 21 children with DS (9 males; Mage = 14;2, SD = 3;4), 21 typically developing children (9 males; Mage = 5;4, SD = 0;6) matched for mental-age (MA), and 21 typically developing children (9 males; Mage = 14;2, SD = 3;6) matched for chronological age (CA). For the matching purpose, a measure of receptive vocabulary, the Peabody Picture Vocabulary

Results

We analyzed the data in a series of mixed and one-way ANOVAs. The Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied in case of missing sphericity in the data. Follow-up statistical comparisons, given the reduced number of participants, were based on non-parametric analyses. The planned contrasts were two-tailed and the p-values were corrected for multiple comparisons analysis using the Bonferroni formula.

Discussion

In the present study, we employed two delayed match-to-sample tasks in order to evaluate visual enumeration skills in children with DS as compared to typically developing individuals matched either for mental or chronological age. In the dots-to-dots match-to-sample task, participants had to enumerate visual sets with numerosities both within and beyond the subitizing range. This task allowed us to systematically evaluate the functioning of both the OTS and the ANS. The most striking result is

Conclusions

In summary, our results suggest that children with DS have a specific deficit in OTS capacity. In contrast, number acuity (supported by the ANS) and the understanding of cardinality, though severely delayed in children with DS with respect to their chronological age, were adequate for their mental age. Thus, mathematics underachievement in children with DS is likely to stem from weakness in these basic numerical skills that are thought to be foundational to mathematical learning.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by grants from the Cariparo Foundation (Excellence Grants 2008) and the European Research Council (# 210922) to M.Z. We are grateful to the children (and their families) who participated in the study. We also thank Erika Torrisi and Elisa Dal Pont for helping in the data collection.

References (50)

  • F. Xu et al.

    Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants

    Cognition

    (2000)
  • H. Abdelahmeed

    Do children with Down syndrome have difficulty in counting and why

    International Journal of Special Education

    (2007)
  • L. Bashash et al.

    Counting skills and number concepts of students with moderate intellectual disabilities

    International Journal of Disability, Development and Education

    (2003)
  • M. Bonato et al.

    Hemispatial neglect: computer-based testing allows more sensitive quantification of attentional disorders and recovery and might lead to better evaluation of rehabilitation

    Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

    (2013)
  • S. Brigstocke et al.

    Number and arithmetic skills in children with Down syndrome

    Down Syndrome: Research and Practice

    (2008)
  • S. Buckley et al.

    The adolescent with Down syndrome: Life for the teenager and for the family

    (1987)
  • V. Camos

    Numerosity discrimination in children with Down syndrome

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2009)
  • S. Carey

    Cognitive foundations of arithmetic: Evolution and ontogenesis

    Mind & Language

    (2001)
  • J. Carr

    Six weeks to twenty-one years old: a longitudinal study of children with Down's syndrome and their families

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (1988)
  • B. Carretti et al.

    The effect of configuration on VSWM performance of Down syndrome individuals

    Journal of Intellectual Disability Research

    (2010)
  • L. Caycho et al.

    Counting by children with Down syndrome

    American Journal on Mental Retardation

    (1991)
  • Cutini, S., Sella, F., & Zorzi, M. (2013). Visual short-term memory load disrupts subitizing limit, submitted for...
  • S. Dehaene

    The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics

    (1997)
  • S. Dehaene et al.

    Log or linear? Distinct intuitions of the number scale in Western and Amazonian indigene cultures

    Science (New York, N.Y.)

    (2008)
  • L.M. Dunn et al.

    Peabody picture vocabulary test

    (1997)
  • Cited by (41)

    • Number estimation in Down syndrome: Cognition or experience?

      2022, Research in Developmental Disabilities
    • Parent-based training of basic number skills in children with Down syndrome using an adaptive computer game

      2021, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      Two core mechanisms represent non-symbolic numerical information (Feigenson, Dehaene, & Spelke, 2004): the Object Tracking System (OTS) provides an object-based exact representation of small sets (up to 4), whereas the Approximate Number System (ANS) encodes larger numerosities (i.e., more than 4) in an analogical and approximate way. Individuals with DS make more errors, even with respect to typically developing children with the same mental age, in comparing small numerosities (in particular 2vs3 and 3vs4; Paterson, Girelli, Butterworth, & Karmiloff-Smith, 2006; Sella, Lanfranchi, & Zorzi, 2013). This evidence suggests that subitizing, that is the accurate perception of small numerosities (up to 4) supported by the OTS, develops atypically in individuals with DS.

    • Perceptual subitizing and conceptual subitizing in Williams syndrome and Down syndrome: Insights from eye movements

      2020, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      This discrepancy could be caused by the different experimental design used by the authors in that study, where participants were asked to compare a target image, that could be either a digit or a set of dots, with another set of dots that could either match or differ for one dot from the target image. The task set-up by Sella et al. (2013), therefore, did not only measure enumeration skills but relied on the participant to compare different stimuli as well and thus, relied heavily on the participants’ working memory abilities, in contrast to the current study. In order to investigate whether there were different scanning behaviours between the groups and whether this affected the enumeration processes, we analysed the eye movements.

    • Mathematical abilities in Down syndrome

      2019, International Review of Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      These differences could influence the respondents’ performance because they may be more or less difficult for them to understand. Instructions aside, even the way stimuli are presented can vary in discrimination tasks: some involve comparing simultaneously-presented stimuli (Abreu-Mendoza & Arias-Trejo, 2015; Belacchi et al., 2014), others present the stimuli sequentially (Sella, Lanfranchi, et al., 2013; Sella, Lucangeli, et al., 2013). Studies on visuospatial working memory have showed that individuals with DS have specific problems in processing simultaneously-as opposed to sequentially-presented information.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text