Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 49, Issue 12, October 2011, Pages 3247-3253
Neuropsychologia

Emotion and ocular responses in Parkinson's disease

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.029Get rights and content

Abstract

Parkinson's disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disease that affects motor, cognitive, and emotional functioning. Previous studies reported reduced skin conductance responses in PD patients, compared to healthy older adults when viewing emotionally arousing pictures. Attenuated skin conductance changes in PD may reflect peripheral autonomic dysfunction (e.g., reduced nerve endings at the sweat gland) or, alternatively, a more central emotional deficit. The aim of the current study was to investigate a second measure of sympathetic arousal—change in pupil dilation. Eye movements, a motor-based correlate of emotional processing, were also assessed. Results indicated that pupil dilation was significantly greater when viewing emotional, compared to neutral pictures for both PD patients and controls. On the other hand, PD patients made fewer fixations with shorter scan paths, particularly when viewing pleasant pictures. These results suggest that PD patients show normal sympathetic arousal to affective stimuli (indexed by pupil diameter), but differences in motor correlates of emotion (eye movements).

Highlights

► Previous study showed muted skin conductance response in Parkinson's patients. ► Skin conductance may be affected by peripheral autonomic dysfunction in PD. ► Pupil dilation was used as an alternative measure of emotional arousal in PD. ► Eye movements served as index of motivated behavior. ► PD group showed normal arousal but abnormal eye movements to emotional pictures.

Introduction

Parkinson's disease is the second most common degenerative disease of the central nervous system, next to Alzheimer's disease, and affects motor, cognitive and emotional functioning. Parkinson's is most often recognized by its cardinal motor symptoms of tremor, rigidity, postural instability, and bradykinesia. Parkinson's patients tend to show motor slowing and reduced movement initiation (Bartels and Leenders, 2009, Bowers et al., 2006a) and the cognitive performance of Parkinson's patients somewhat mirrors the pattern of motor functioning, with a neuropsychological profile characterized by slowed processing speed on frontal-based cognitive tasks (e.g. Schneider, 2007, Taylor and Saint-Cyr, 1995). These cognitive symptoms are thought to be related to dysfunction of frontal–subcortical basal ganglia circuitry.

The nature of emotional dysfunction in PD is less well characterized. Parkinson's patients experience high rates of apathy and depression and recent research suggests that apathy may be a core feature of Parkinson's disease, with estimates of apathy in PD ranging from 38% to 51% across studies (Isella et al., 2002, Pluck and Brown, 2002, Sockeel et al., 2006, Starkstein et al., 1992). A recent study that compared rates of depression, apathy, and combined apathy and depression in Parkinson's disease and a comparative movement disorder population, dystonia, found that 29% of PD patients endorsed clinically significant apathy without depression, whereas no dystonic patients endorsed significant apathy in the absence of depression (Kirsch-Darrow, Fernandez, Marsiske, Okun, & Bowers, 2006).

Nonetheless, the specific mechanism underlying emotional dysfunction in PD remains unclear. A number of studies have reported that PD patients have abnormal recognition of facial emotion (Ariatti et al., 2008, Delaveau et al., 2009, Sprengelmeyer et al., 2003, Tessitore et al., 2002). However, few studies have employed experimental methods using a wide range of emotional stimuli in order to investigate the nature of emotional processes in PD. Of those that have, results have been mixed. Bowers, Miller, Mikos et al. (2006b) found that Parkinson's patients demonstrated a blunted startle eye-blink response compared to controls while viewing unpleasant pictures and Miller, Okun, Marsiske, Fennell, and Bowers (2009) a reported a trend such that healthy controls showed increased startle potentiation to high vs. low arousing stimuli, whereas Parkinson's patients did not. Based on these findings, Miller et al. speculated that Parkinson's patients might be hypoaroused to emotional stimuli.

This hypothesis is consistent with preliminary findings from our laboratory that Parkinson's patients showed a blunted skin conductance response when viewing emotional (pleasant and unpleasant) pictures compared to healthy older adults (Bowers et al., 2008). While one interpretation of these findings is that Parkinson's patients are hypoaroused to emotional stimuli, these findings are complicated by the fact that Parkinson's patients also have damage to the peripheral autonomic nervous system, including reduced nerve endings at the sweat glands of the palm (Dabby et al., 2006). Thus, peripheral autonomic dysfunction could be an important factor mediating reduced skin conductance responses. The aim of the current study was to investigate the utility of measuring changes in pupil diameter as an alternative measure of emotional arousal in PD.

Investigations of pupillary changes have concluded that pupil constriction, including the initial light reflex following onset of visual stimulation, is predominantly controlled via parasympathetic input to the sphincter muscle from the Edinger Westphal nucleus. On the other hand, pupil dilation is predominantly controlled via sympathetic input to the dilator muscle from the thoracic cell columns in the spinal cord. Pupil dilation can result from either direct sympathetic input, which is modulated by noradrenergic brain stem nuclei, the hypothalamus, and the central nucleus of the amygdala, or from the inhibition of parasympathetic input to the sphincter muscle, primarily mediated by the reticular formation, locus coeruleus and other direct and indirect cortical pathways (Gilzenrat et al., 2010, Lowenstein, 1955, Steinhauer et al., 2004).

While a number of studies have explored the neural mechanisms underlying cognitive effects on the pupil (e.g. Steinhauer et al., 2000, Steinhauer et al., 2004), less attention in the last decade has been directed towards elucidating the exact mechanisms underlying the effect of emotional arousal on the pupil. Early studies by Hess and Polt in the 1960s reported bidirectional effects on pupil dilation depending on the pleasantness of the emotional stimulus. These findings were difficult to replicate, perhaps due to imprecise measurement, lack of statistical analyses, and small numbers of stimuli and participants (see Bradley, Miccoli, Escrig, & Lang, 2008).

More recently, Bradley and colleagues (Bradley et al., 2008) utilized a wide range of standardized emotional picture stimuli and found that, rather than bidirectional effects, pupil dilation was significantly greater when participants viewed both pleasant and unpleasant pictures, compared to neutral pictures. Following the initial parasympathetically-mediated light reflex, pupil changes covaried with skin conductance responses in these healthy college students, prompting the conclusion that pupil dilation is an index of sympathetic activation during emotional picture processing. This response may be driven by enhanced sympathetic input to the pupil via modulatory input from the central nucleus of the amygdala and the hypothalamus (see Ranson & Clark, 1959 as cited in White & Depue, 1999). Bradley, Houbova, Miccoli, Costa, and Lang (2010) also found that healthy adults make a greater number of voluntary visual fixations, and with longer scan paths, when viewing pictures with emotional, compared to neutral content. These differences were interpreted as reflecting increased information seeking in a motivationally relevant context, as a prelude to selecting/initiating an appropriate defensive or appetitive action.

The current study measured both pupil diameter and ocular movements as indices of emotional processing in PD patients and healthy older adults during affective picture processing. Prior studies have reported a variety of saccadic deficits in Parkinson's disease. For instance, one study concluded that PD patients show normal voluntary saccade movements but impaired reflexive movements (Yoshida, Yamada, & Matsuzaki, 2002) whereas another found the opposite results (Briand, Strallow, Hening, Poizner, & Sereno, 1999). Studies have also found that PD patients show hyper-reflexive saccades (Fielding et al., 2006, van Koningsbruggen et al., 2009). Finally, a recent study (Clark, Neargarder, & Cronin-Golomb, 2010) found no difference in number or duration of visual fixations for PD patients when viewing emotional faces.

Previous investigations of pupil motility in PD have consistently found that Parkinson's patients show a reduced amplitude of the initial light reflex (Beaumont et al., 1987, Granholm et al., 2003, Harris, 1991, Micieli et al., 1991), but no differences in the maximum dilation during dark adaptation (Micieli et al., 1991) or in response to tropicamide, an acetylcholine antagonist which blocks the parasympathetic input to the sphincter muscle (Granholm et al., 2003). Therefore, while the parasympathetically mediated initial light reflex may be compromised in PD, there is no significant evidence that sympathetic input to the pupil is jeopardized by PD. In the current study, visual parameters were selected to elicit a measurable light reflex following picture onset. We expected that PD patients would show attenuation of this parasympathetically mediated reflex, in line with previous studies. Because effects of emotional arousal on pupil dilation are likely to be mediated by sympathetic nervous system activity (Bradley et al., 2008), measuring pupil diameter following the light reflex during emotional picture viewing provides an index of the integrity of sympathetic activation in emotional processing in PD.

Because previous studies have hypothesized that disrupted emotional processing in PD may be related to amygdalar dysfunction (Bowers et al., 2006a, Bowers et al., 2006b, Tessitore et al., 2002) one hypothesis is that PD patients will show reduced emotional reactivity when viewing emotional stimuli for both pupil dilation and eye movements. An alternative hypothesis is that, if emotional dysfunction in PD is driven by disruption of higher cortical circuitry with the basal ganglia, then PD patients and controls might exhibit similar sympathetic arousal, measured by pupil dilation, whereas group differences would be most pronounced in the motor system, as indexed by eye movements.

Section snippets

Participants

Fifteen nondemented Parkinson's patients and fifteen healthy older adults participated in the current study. After data collection, one patient and three controls were excluded due to loss of pupil discrimination for over 25% of trials, resulting in a final N of 14 PD patients and 12 healthy controls. Parkinson's patients were recruited from the University of Florida Movement Disorders Clinic and had all been previously examined by a movement disorders specialist and met brain bank criteria for

Pupil dilation

Fig. 1 shows the average change in pupil diameter from a one second pre-picture baseline for each group, across the six-second interval of picture viewing (across all trial types). There were no significant difference in terms of baseline pupil diameter between PD patients (mean = 4.76 mm) and control participants (mean = 4.45 mm), t(24) = .68, p = .50.

On the other hand, as illustrated in Fig. 1, an independent samples t-test indicated that the initial light reflex occurring following picture onset was

Discussion

Both Parkinson's patients and healthy controls showed increased pupil dilation while viewing emotional (pleasant or unpleasant) compared to neutral pictures. These data suggest intact sympathetic arousal when viewing emotional pictures in PD patients, and replicate the pattern of pupillary reactions observed when healthy young adults view emotional and neutral pictures (e.g. Bradley et al., 2008). These data suggest that previously reported SCR blunting (Bowers et al., 2008) may be related to

Acknowledgements

This project was completed as part of a master's thesis (J. Dietz) at the University of Florida. The research was supported in part by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (P50 MH72850) to PJ Lang & MM Bradley at the Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention (CSEA) at the University of Florida. Support was also provided by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (RO1-NS05063 to D. Bowers, K23-NS044997 to M.S. Okun) and the UF National Parkinson Foundation

References (54)

  • S. Steinhauer et al.

    Sympathetic and parasympathetic innervation of pupillary dilation during sustained processing

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2004)
  • A.E. Taylor et al.

    The neuropsychology of Parkinson's disease

    Brain and Cognition

    (1995)
  • M.G. van Koningsbruggen et al.

    Impaired control of the oculomotor reflexes in Parkinson's disease

    Neuropsychologia

    (2009)
  • G. Alexander et al.

    Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex

    Annual Review of Neuroscience

    (1986)
  • A. Ariatti et al.

    Recognition of emotions from visual and prosodic cues in Parkinson's disease

    Neurological Sciences

    (2008)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Manual for the beck depression inventory-II

    (1996)
  • S.M. Beaumont et al.

    The pupillary light reflex in mild Parkinson's disease

    Clinical Vision Sciences

    (1987)
  • D. Bowers et al.

    Apathy and psychophysiologic blunting in Parkinson disease

  • D. Bowers et al.

    Faces of emotion in Parkinsons disease: Micro-expressivity and bradykinesia during voluntary facial expressions

    Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society

    (2006)
  • D. Bowers et al.

    Startling facts about emotion in Parkinson's disease: Blunted reactivity to aversive stimuli

    Brain: A Journal of Neurology

    (2006)
  • H. Braak et al.

    Amygdala pathology in Parkinson's disease

    Acta Neuropathology

    (1994)
  • M.M. Bradley et al.

    Scan patterns when viewing natural scenes: Emotion, complexity, and repetition

    Psychophysiology

    (2010)
  • M.M. Bradley et al.

    The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation

    Psychophysiology

    (2008)
  • K.A. Briand et al.

    Control of voluntary and reflexive saccades in Parkinson's disease

    Experimental Brain Research

    (1999)
  • R. Dabby et al.

    Skin biopsy for assessment of autonomic denervation in Parkinson's disease

    Journal of Neural Transmission

    (2006)
  • P. Delaveau et al.

    Dopaminergic modulation of amygdala activity during emotion recognition in patients with Parkinson disease

    Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology

    (2009)
  • S. Fahn et al.

    Unified Parkinson's disease rating scale

  • Cited by (28)

    • Measuring emotion recognition by people with Parkinson's disease using eye-tracking with dynamic facial expressions

      2020, Journal of Neuroscience Methods
      Citation Excerpt :

      Eye-tracking could therefore be a useful tool for investigating the recognition of dynamic and static expressions in PD. However, neuropathological changes in PD can result in altered eye movements, such as longer fixations, delayed saccade initiation and reduced saccade amplitude and velocity (e.g., Archibald et al., 2013; Chan et al., 2005; Dietz et al., 2011; Kennard et al., 1994). Although Clark et al. (2010) found that visual scanning and accuracy in a static emotion recognition task were unrelated to antisaccade performance in participants with PD and age-matched controls, it is possible that oculomotor abnormalities impact upon the ability to efficiently utilise motion information from dynamic expressions.

    • Eye gaze behavior during affective picture viewing: Effects of motivational significance, gender, age, and repeated exposure

      2019, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      The valence hypothesis is supported by a study reporting more fixations and longer scanpath for unpleasant than pleasant images of similar normative arousal (Blakemore, Rieger, & Vuilleumier, 2016). However, Bradley et al. (Bradley et al., 2011; Dietz et al., 2011) observed no significant differences in eye gaze movements between pleasant and unpleasant pictures of similar normative arousal. Furthermore, Madan et al. (2018) found that pictures’ mean pleasantness as rated by the participants did not correlate significantly with number of fixations and correlated positively rather than negatively with scanpath length.

    • Beyond emotion recognition deficits: A theory guided analysis of emotion processing in Huntington's disease

      2017, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      To our knowledge, no research in HD has examined the motivational changes and physiological responses components of the CPM. Research in PD, however, conducted a small subset of possible physiological measurements during the presentation of emotional stimuli (Bowers et al., 2006; Dietz et al., 2011; Miller et al., 2009), including pupil dilation in response to emotional pictures and the startle eye-blink response to unpleasant stimuli. In the following sections, we review clinical research conducted in HD using the methodological approaches described in the previous section (Section 3), to offer a comprehensive, theory-based understanding of emotion processing in HD.

    • Disruption of pupil size modulation correlates with voluntary motor preparation deficits in Parkinson's disease

      2016, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      The combination of pupil size and saccadic eye movements in behavioral paradigms provides a possibility to index both cognitive and autonomic functions, potentially producing more effective biomarkers for various diseases in clinical investigation. One study has combined measurements of pupil size and eye movements to examine emotional processing in PD, highlighting the possibility of this approach (Dietz et al., 2011). The anti-saccade task has been used extensively to investigate voluntary control of executive functions in healthy as well as in clinical populations (Hutton and Ettinger, 2006; Munoz et al., 1998; Munoz and Everling, 2004; Everling and Fischer, 1998).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text