Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 159, 1 October 2017, Pages 146-158
NeuroImage

Breakdown of long-range temporal correlations in brain oscillations during general anesthesia

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.07.047Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Long-range temporal correlations (LRTCs) analyzed in macaque ECoG recordings.

  • Wakefulness: robust LRTCs form a posterior-anterior topographic gradient.

  • Anesthesia: weak LRTCs and breakdown of the gradient.

  • Weak LRTCs during anesthesia might indicate disrupted temporal integration of information.

Abstract

Consciousness has been hypothesized to emerge from complex neuronal dynamics, which prevails when brain operates in a critical state. Evidence supporting this hypothesis comes mainly from studies investigating neuronal activity on a short time-scale of seconds. However, a key aspect of criticality is presence of scale-free temporal dependencies occurring across a wide range of time-scales. Indeed, robust long-range temporal correlations (LRTCs) are found in neuronal oscillations during conscious states, but it is not known how LRTCs are affected by loss of consciousness. To further test a relation between critical dynamics and consciousness, we investigated LRTCs in electrocorticography signals recorded from four macaque monkeys during resting wakefulness and general anesthesia induced by various anesthetics (ketamine, medetomidine, or propofol). Detrended Fluctuation Analysis was used to estimate LRTCs in amplitude fluctuations (envelopes) of band-pass filtered signals. We demonstrate two main findings. First, during conscious states all lateral cortical regions are characterized by significant LRTCs of alpha-band activity (7–14 Hz). LRTCs are stronger in the eyes-open than eyes-closed state, but in both states they form a spatial gradient, with anterior brain regions exhibiting stronger LRTCs than posterior regions. Second, we observed a substantial decrease of LRTCs during loss of consciousness, the magnitude of which was associated with the baseline (i.e. pre-anesthesia) state of the brain. Specifically, brain regions characterized by strongest LRTCs during a wakeful baseline exhibited greatest decreases during anesthesia (i.e. “the rich got poorer”), which consequently disturbed the posterior-anterior gradient. Therefore, our results suggest that general anesthesia affects mainly brain areas characterized by strongest LRTCs during wakefulness, which might account for lack of capacities for extensive temporal integration during loss of consciousness.

Introduction

Brain activity, both task-related and spontaneously generated, is characterized by complex spatio-temporal dynamics. It has been proposed that understanding brain as a system operating in a critical state might provide a theoretical framework allowing comprehensive description of neuronal activity (review: Chialvo, 2007, Chialvo, 2010, Shew and Plenz, 2013, Beggs and Timme, 2012). A critical state can occur in complex systems composed of multiple interacting elements. When balanced interactions among the elements keep a system operating at the verge of order and randomness a plethora of non-trivial features can be observed (in contrast to rather trivial states of complete order and randomness). A hallmark of criticality is lack of any characteristic scale in system's activity (i.e. scale-free dynamics), which is indicated by a power-law scaling (Beggs and Timme, 2012).

First evidence for brain operating in a critical state came from studies investigating “neuronal avalanches”, which were defined as temporally consistent bursts (clusters) of activations occurring simultaneously at several brain locations. Neuronal avalanches were first investigated in LFP recordings, with activations defined based on deflections of LFP signals. Scale-free avalanches, with their size and duration following a power law, were observed both in vitro (Beggs and Plenz, 2003, Beggs and Plenz, 2004) and in vivo (Gireesh and Plenz, 2008, Petermann et al., 2009, Hahn et al., 2010). Further studies confirmed that similar scale-free dynamics can be found in avalanches defined based on non-invasively recorded MEG (Shriki et al., 2013) or fMRI data (He, 2011, Tagliazucchi et al., 2012).

But evidence for brain criticality is not limited to studies investigating “neuronal avalanches”. Another line of research focused on long-range temporal correlations (LRTCs), which are defined as a power-law decay of an autocorrelation function and thus indicate that a signal exhibits scale-free patterns in the temporal domain (in the form of 1/f spectrum). LRTCs can be thought to reflect a long “memory” of a signal and a simultaneous modulation across a range of time-scales. Robust LRTCs were found in amplitude modulation of oscillatory brain activity (Linkenkaer-Hansen et al., 2001, Linkenkaer-Hansen and Nikulin, 2004) and in temporal progression of EEG topographical voltage patterns (so called “microstates”; Van de Ville et al., 2010, Gschwind et al., 2015). Importantly, even before describing LRTCs in brain activity, their presence had been observed in behavioral patterns of humans and other species (Gilden et al., 1995, Kello et al., 2010). Recent studies demonstrate links between these various scales of analysis by showing that LRTCs in neuronal oscillations (meso-scale) are related to “neuronal avalanches” in spiking activity (micro-scale; Poil et al., 2012) and, at the same time, to temporal dependencies observed in behavior (macro-scale; Palva et al., 2013, Smit et al., 2013).

Operating in a critical regime provides functional benefits for a system, as it allows balanced propagation of external and internal perturbations and is related to maximization of information storage and transfer (Shew et al., 2009, Shew et al., 2011, Marinazzo et al., 2014). Crucially, the qualities of critical systems overlap with features postulated necessary for emergence of consciousness from brain networks (Tononi, 2004, Seth, 2009, Dehaene and Changeux, 2011, Tononi et al., 2016). Therefore, it has been hypothesized that consciousness occurs when brain operates in (or close to) a critical state, whereas changes in the state consciousness are associated with departure from criticality towards either sub- or super-critical states (Chialvo, 2007, Werner, 2013, Tagliazucchi et al., 2013, Tagliazucchi et al., 2016; Carhart-Harris et al., 2014). Yet, evidence supporting this hypothesis is so far scarce and inconclusive. Some studies investigating LFP recordings found that, indeed, neuronal avalanches loose their scale-free features during loss of consciousness suggesting a departure from the critical state (Ribeiro et al., 2010, Priesemann et al., 2013), but others reported that conscious and unconscious states do not differ in this respect (Dehghani et al., 2012). However, a main limitation of these LFP studies was sparse and selective data sampling by electrodes arrays. Conversely, by analyzing neuronal avalanches estimated from voltage imaging data (recorded from mice and characterized by superb spatial sampling and resolution) Scott et al. (2014) and Fagerholm et al. (2016) robustly demonstrated critical dynamics during wakefulness and departure from criticality under anesthesia.

However, in contrast to neuronal avalanches, neurophysiological LRTCs have not been systematically investigated in different states of consciousness. We are only aware of two fMRI studies which revealed loss of LRTCs in blood–oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) time-series during NREM sleep (Tagliazucchi et al., 2013) and propofol anesthesia (Tagliazucchi et al., 2016). Therefore, we set out to investigate LRTCs in neuronal activity during resting wakefulness and loss of consciousness caused by general anesthesia. Electrocorticographic (ECoG) data recorded from macaque monkeys were analyzed with Detrended Fluctuation Analysis (DFA; Peng et al., 1994), in order to estimate LRTCs in amplitude modulation of brain oscillatory activity (Fig. 1). We hypothesized to observe robust LRTCs during resting wakefulness and a breakdown of long-range temporal dependencies during loss of consciousness. To test these hypotheses we: first, investigated how general anesthesia affects LRTC; second, conducted detailed analyses of LRTCs' topography during wakefulness; and third, investigated topographic patterns of anesthesia-induced changes in LRTCs.

Section snippets

Results

The ECoG recordings used in the present study were published as a part of the open-access Neurotycho database (Nagasaka et al., 2011). In the original study ECoG signals were acquired from 4 macaque monkeys using arrays of 128 electrodes covering the whole lateral part of the left hemisphere (Fig. S1). Altogether 20 experimental sessions were chosen for analysis. Importantly, 4 different anesthetic agents were used across sessions to induce general anesthesia: propofol, ketamine, medetomidine,

Discussion

In the present study we investigated long-range temporal correlations (LRTCs) in electrocorticographic (ECoG) data recorded from four macaque monkeys during wakefulness and general anesthesia. LRTCs reflect scale-free temporal modulation of activity and thus are considered a hallmark of systems operating in a critical state. Here we focused on investigating LRTCs in amplitude of the alpha-band oscillations and we demonstrate two main findings. First, during resting wakefulness robust LRTCs were

Experimental procedures

The dataset analyzed in the present study was recorded at the RIKEN Institute (Japan) and it is publicly available from the Neurotycho database (http://neurotycho.org/; Nagasaka et al., 2011). The experimental protocol was approved by the RIKEN Ethics Committee.

One macaca mulatta (S) and three macaca fuscata monkeys (G, K, C) took part in the study. Altogether 20 experimental sessions were chosen for the analysis, each session conducted on a separate day (see: Table S1). Four different

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Author contributions

DK: data analysis, interpretation of results, revising the manuscript; MK: supervision of data analysis, interpretation of results, revising the manuscript; AM: interpretation of results, revising the manuscript; MB: conception and design, supervision of data analysis, interpretation of results, drafting and revising the manuscript.

Acknowledgments

MB was supported by START stipend from the Foundation for Polish Science (START 009.2016), Iuventus Plus grant from the Polish Ministry of Science and Higher Education (082/IP3/2016/74), and Sonata grant from the National Science Centre Poland (2015/17/D/HS6/00269). MK was supported by a Statutory Grant from the Polish Ministry of Science and Higher Education to the Faculty of Physics of University of Warsaw. We thank Alexandros Goulas for help with assigning electrodes to anatomical modules.

References (93)

  • J.W. Kantelhardt et al.

    Detecting long-range correlations with detrended fluctuation analysis

    Phys. A Stat. Mech. its Appl.

    (2001)
  • C.T. Kello et al.

    Scaling laws in cognitive sciences

    Trends Cognit. Sci.

    (2010)
  • J.R. King et al.

    Information sharing in the brain indexes consciousness in noncommunicative patients

    Curr. Biol.

    (2013)
  • L. Mudrik et al.

    Information integration without awareness

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2014)
  • V. Nikulin et al.

    Long-range temporal correlations in alpha and beta oscillations: effect of arousal level and test–retest reliability

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2004)
  • P.D. Oldham

    A note on the analysis of repeated measurements of the same subjects

    J. chronic Dis.

    (1962)
  • O. Ossmy et al.

    The timescale of perceptual evidence integration can be adapted to the environment

    Curr. Biol.

    (2013)
  • S. Sarasso et al.

    Consciousness and complexity during unresponsiveness induced by propofol, xenon, and ketamine

    Curr. Biol.

    (2015)
  • G. Werner

    Consciousness related brain processes viewed in the framework of phase space dynamics, criticality, and the renormalization group

    Chaos, Solit. Fractals

    (2013)
  • L.M. Alonso et al.

    Dynamical criticality during induction of anesthesia in human ECoG recordings

    Front. Neural Circ.

    (2014)
  • J.P. Archie

    Mathematic coupling of data: a common source of error

    Ann. Surg.

    (1981)
  • C. Bedard et al.

    Does the 1/f frequency scaling of brain signals reflect self-organized critical states?

    Phys. Rev. Lett.

    (2006)
  • J.M. Beggs et al.

    Neuronal avalanches in neocortical circuits

    J. Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • J.M. Beggs et al.

    Neuronal avalanches are diverse and precise activity patterns that are stable for many hours in cortical slice cultures

    J. Neurosci.

    (2004)
  • J.M. Beggs et al.

    Being critical of criticality in the brain

    Front. Physiol.

    (2012)
  • T.E. Behrens et al.

    Learning the value of information in an uncertain world

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • T.A. Bekinschtein et al.

    Neural signature of the conscious processing of auditory regularities

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.

    (2009)
  • R.L. Carhart-Harris et al.

    The entropic brain: a theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs,

    Front. Hum. Neurosci.

    (2014)
  • A.G. Casali et al.

    A theoretically based index of consciousness independent of sensory processing and behavior

    Sci. Transl. Med.

    (2013)
  • Z. Chen et al.

    Effect of nonstationarities on detrended fluctuation analysis

    Phys. Rev. E

    (2002)
  • S. Chennu et al.

    Brain connectivity dissociates responsiveness from drug exposure during propofol-induced transitions of consciousness

    PLoS Comput. Biol.

    (2016)
  • D.R. Chialvo

    The brain near the edge

  • D.R. Chialvo

    Emergent complex neural dynamics

    Nat. Phys.

    (2010)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences

    (1988)
  • Matthew H. Davis

    Dissociating speech perception and comprehension at reduced levels of awareness

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2007)
  • N. Dehghani et al.

    Avalanche Analysis from Multi-electrode Ensemble Recordings in Cat, Monkey and Human Cerebral Cortex during Wakefulness and Sleep

    (2012)
  • M.M. Farbood et al.

    The neural processing of hierarchical structure in music and speech at different timescales

    Front. Neurosci.

    (2015)
  • E.D. Fagerholm et al.

    Cortical entropy, mutual information and scale-free dynamics in waking mice

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2016)
  • T.H. Fitzgerald et al.

    Cross-frequency coupling within and between the human thalamus and neocortex

    Front. Hum. Neurosci.

    (2013)
  • N. Furl et al.

    Cross-frequency power coupling between hierarchically organized face-selective areas

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2014)
  • D.L. Gilden et al.

    1/f noise in human cognition

    Science

    (1995)
  • E.D. Gireesh et al.

    Neuronal avalanches organize as nested theta- and beta/gamma-oscillations during development of cortical layer 2/3

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2008)
  • A. Goulas et al.

    The strength of weak connections in the macaque cortico-cortical network

    Brain Struct. Funct.

    (2015)
  • G. Hahn et al.

    Neuronal avalanches in spontaneous activity in vivo

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2010)
  • R. Hardstone et al.

    Detrended fluctuation analysis: a scale-free view on neuronal oscillations

    Front. Physiol.

    (2012)
  • U. Hasson et al.

    A hierarchy of temporal receptive windows in human cortex

    J. Neurosci.

    (2008)
  • Cited by (26)

    • Dynamic alpha-gamma phase-amplitude coupling signatures during sevoflurane-induced loss and recovery of consciousness

      2022, Neuroscience Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      One of the most common assumptions for how the LOC is generated from the perspective of connectivity is that long-range communication between cortical regions (especially the frontal-parietal) is disrupted. For instance, Krzemin´ski et al. (2017) found a substantial decrease in long-range temporal correlations during LOC. Using intracortical recordings from macaque monkeys, Schroeder et al. (2016) showed that ketamine anesthesia inhibits information transmission among structurally linked cortical regions.

    • Using non-invasive neuroimaging to enhance the care, well-being and experimental outcomes of laboratory non-human primates (monkeys)

      2021, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      The choice of the anesthetic agent is key for task-based and resting-state fMRI experiments. As an alternative to inhalant anesthetics, injectable agents such as ketamine or propofol, and adjuncts such as medetomidine are attractive in providing a satisfactory depth of anesthesia, with relatively quick onset of action, and reversible smooth recovery of the animals (Bertrand et al., 2016; Bertrand et al., 2017; Bola et al., 2018; Fahlman et al., 2006; Hess et al., 2012; Krzemiński et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2010; Liu et al., 2015b; Lugo-Roman et al., 2010; Pulley et al., 2004; Settle et al., 2010; Sun et al., 2003; Young et al., 1999). Propofol is an injectable anesthetic with a rapid mechanism of action that is widely used in humans (Amico et al., 2014; Boveroux et al., 2010; Golkowski et al., 2019; Gómez et al., 2013; Huang et al., 2018; Hudetz, 2012; Hudetz et al., 2015; Jordan et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2013; Li et al., 2019), and increasingly used in fMRI experiments in animal models, including rodents (Grandjean et al., 2014) and NHPs (Barttfeld et al., 2015; Bola et al., 2018; Ishizawa et al., 2016; Krzemiński et al., 2017; Uhrig et al., 2016; Uhrig et al., 2018).

    • Long-range temporal correlations in the brain distinguish conscious wakefulness from induced unconsciousness

      2018, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Our study is the first to use DFA specifically on band-limited oscillation amplitude signals (envelopes) in humans during wakefulness and sevoflurane-induced unconsciousness. In a recent study, Krzemiński et al. (2017) used DFA on the amplitude of local field potentials (LFP) in monkeys and observed a decrease of LRTC during unconsciousness induced by anesthetics. The discrepancies between their results and ours could be due to a number of reasons.

    • Loss of consciousness is related to hyper-correlated gamma-band activity in anesthetized macaques and sleeping humans

      2018, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      A rationale behind this line of work is testing directly neuronal mechanisms hypothesized to give rise to conscious experiences (Seth, 2009; Tononi et al., 2016) and consequently providing more explanatorily powerful correlates of consciousness. A now well-established finding is that complexity of electrophysiological signals (both spontaneous and event-related) decrease during unconscious states (Casali et al., 2013; Alonso et al., 2014; Schartner et al., 2015, 2016; Solovey et al., 2015; Tajima et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2017; Krzemiński et al., 2017). Therefore, the excessive inter-dependence of gamma-band activity during LOC fits well with these previous results as it might reflect suppression of information transfer and departure from a critical regime (Chialvo, 2010).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text