Elsevier

Landscape and Urban Planning

Volume 168, December 2017, Pages 31-37
Landscape and Urban Planning

Research Paper
Acoustic masking of soniferous species of the St-Lawrence lowlands

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2017.09.015Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Estimated acoustic masking between human- and animal-induced sounds in three soundscape contexts.

  • Measured acoustic overlap in the timing, frequencies, amplitudes of signaling birds, anurans and orthopterans.

  • Acoustic masking is unlikely for most species, but anurans in an urban context.

  • Temporal overlap between anthrophony and biophony increased along the urbanization gradient for birds and anurans.

  • Attention must be paid to other factors that covary with the amount of noise produced by human activities.

Abstract

How much of the acoustic resource shared by vocalizing animal species is masked by human-induced noise has become a central question in the emerging field of soundscape ecology. The goal of our study was to evaluate the joint probability that masking could occur for anuran, bird, or stridulating orthopteran species along the spectral, spatial and temporal dimensions of the acoustic space. We evaluated acoustic overlap in the dominant frequencies, absolute amplitudes (emitter-receiver distances), and time-series (temporal match-mismatch) of human- and animal-induced sounds in three landscape settings: urban, peri-urban and rural. Acoustic overlap was evaluated at the day scale during the active vocalizing period of each taxon. Our results suggest that acoustic masking is unlikely for a vast majority of species in the three taxonomic groups; ranging from 5–7% for anurans to 3–4% for birds and less than 1% for orthopterans. However, in urban contexts, acoustic masking could be a selective force when coupled to other factors that covary with the amount of noise produced by human activities, such as habitat loss and degradation.

Introduction

Communication in the animal kingdom is used by individuals to attract females during the reproductive stage, defend territories and signal food supply to conspecifics (Bradbury & Verenchamps, 2011; Gerhardt & Huber, 2002). Communication can be achieved with visual, olfactory or acoustic signals (Endler, 1993; Rekwot, Ogwu, Oyedipe, & Sekoni, 2001). In noisy environments, acoustic communication can be masked, thus forcing individuals to invest energy into compensatory mechanisms, such as shifting call frequencies (Lengagne, 2008, Slabbekoorn and Peet, 2003), vocalizing louder (Brumm, 2004; Cynx, Lewis, Travel, & Tse, 1998; Penna & Hamilton-West, 2006), or moving away from the noise source (Francis, Ortega, & Cruz, 2011; Goutte, Dubois, & Legendre, 2013). Previous studies have reported examples of adaptations to acoustic masking in anurans (Cunnington & Fahrig, 2010), birds (Hu & Cardoso, 2010; Wood and Yezerinac, 2006) and insects (Morley, Jones, & Radford, 2014; Shieh, Liang, & Chiu, 2012). However, it is not clear if a majority of species in these three taxonomic groups are affected by acoustic masking. For example, a recent meta-analysis by Roca et al. (2016) showed that only small-bodied bird species singing at low frequencies (<3 kHz) increased their dominant frequencies when exposed to anthropogenic noise, whereas anurans were less prone to such shifts.

To estimate the global importance of acoustic masking events, it is important to know the probability of spectral, spatial, and temporal overlap between human-induced and animal-induced sounds. Acoustic masking events occur if the communication signals are emitted i) at the same frequency (spectral overlap), ii) close enough to the noise source (spatial overlap), and iii) concurrently to human-induced sounds (temporal overlap) (Brumm and Slabbekoorn, 2005, Patterson and Green, 1978, Slabbekoorn et al., 2010). Human-induced sounds generally have low dominant frequencies and, as such, should not impact every vocalizing organisms equally (Hu & Cardoso, 2010; Slabbekoorn & Peet,2003). Anurans calls could be more susceptible to acoustic masking due to their spectral overlap with human-induced sounds (e.g., Cunnington & Fahrig, 2010). Hu and Cardoso (2009) similarly argued that birds singing at high frequency should predominate in urban ecosystems because their signaling frequency is out of the range of human-induced sounds.

Acoustic masking can be particularly important in urban settings because species did not coevolve with human-induced sounds (Feng & Schul, 2007). Although, data on the intensity level and the frequency spectrum of urban sounds are relatively easy to obtain (see Wood & Yezerinac, 2006; Wei, van Renterghem, De Coensel, & Botteldooren, 2016), little is known of the amount of temporal overlap between human-induced sounds and animal-induced sounds. Studies that explored the temporal patterns of vocal organisms were mostly conducted in natural ecosystems (e.g., Krause, Gage, & Woo, 2011; Rodriguez et al., 2014) or did not explicitly consider the anthrophony component of the soundscape (e.g., Gage & Axel 2014; Gage, Joo, Kasten, Fox, & Biswas, 2015). Hence, bird and anuran individuals exchanging acoustic signals close to a noise source, and at low frequencies, may avoid acoustic masking by vocalizing when temporal overlap is minimized during the day (Cartwright, Taylor, Wilson, & Chow-Fraser, 2014; Warren, Katti, Ermann, & Brazel, 2006).

The aim of the study was to evaluate the joint probability that acoustic masking could occur for anuran, bird, or stridulating orthopteran species along the spectral, spatial, and temporal dimensions of the acoustic space. We evaluated acoustic overlap in the dominant frequencies, absolute amplitudes (i.e., emitter-receiver distances), and acoustic patterns (i.e., temporal match-mismatch) of human- and animal-induced sounds in three soundscape contexts: urban, peri-urban, and agricultural. We paid particular attention to the daily temporal overlap between human-induced sounds (anthrophony) and animal-induced sounds (biophony), for which no estimates currently exist.

Section snippets

Material and methods

To estimate the likelihood of an acoustic masking event, we independently evaluated the probability of spectral, spatial, and temporal overlap between human-induced sounds (anthrophony) and animal-induced sounds (biophony). We estimated the probability that species of a given taxonomic group t could experience an acoustic masking event when exposed to anthrophony with the following equation:AMt=SpeOt·SpaOt·TOt,where AM is acoustic masking, SpeO is spectral overlap probability, SpaO is spatial

Spectral overlap

The frequency spectrum of anuran calls showed a large spectral overlap with human-induced sounds, while the spectrum of orthopteran stridulations barely overlapped (Fig. 1). The probability of a spectral overlap between human- and animal-induced sounds was 32%, 26% and 7%, for anuran, bird and orthopteran species, respectively (Table 1). Using the weighted mean frequency did not change the spectral overlap ranking of each taxonomic group, with overlap probabilities ranging from 49% for anurans,

Discussion

An increasing number of studies have raised concerns about the impact of human-induced sounds and acoustic masking events on animal communication and, more generally, on animal behavior (Brumm and Slabbekoorn, 2005, Morley et al., 2014, Roca et al., 2016, Warren et al., 2006). The present study takes a broader perspective and asks whether these examples should be considered the rule or the exception. Our results suggest that acoustic masking per se is unlikely for a majority of species in the

Conclusion

What is the likelihood that a given individual from a regional species pool will experience an event of acoustic masking? Answering this fundamental question is needed to comprehend the impact of human-induced sounds on vocalizing species and to prioritize conservation efforts. We herein provided conservative, upper-bound, estimates of acoustic masking for three taxonomic groups, which suggest that human-induced sounds may not be of major concern in a majority of cases of impaired acoustic

Acknowledgements

The authors acknowledge the financial support provided by the Fonds de recherche du Québec – Nature et technologies (FRQNT) to L.D., the financial support provided by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) to R.P. and the grants from the Groupe de recherche interuniversitaire en limnologie et en environnement aquatique (GRIL).

References (65)

  • C.A. Martin et al.

    Habitat geometry, a step toward general bird community assembly rules in mature forests

    Forest Ecology and Management

    (2016)
  • D. Patón et al.

    Tolerance to noise in 91 bird species from 27 urban gardens of Iberian Peninsula

    Landscape and Urban Planning

    (2012)
  • R.D. Patterson et al.

    Auditory masking

  • A. Rodriguez et al.

    Temporal and spatial variability of animal sound within a neotropical forest

    Ecological Informatics

    (2014)
  • H. Slabbekoorn et al.

    A noisy spring: The impact of globally rising underwater sound levels on fish

    Trends in Ecology and Evolution

    (2010)
  • P.S. Warren et al.

    Urban bioacoustics: It’s not just noise

    Animal Behaviour

    (2006)
  • W. Wei et al.

    Dynamic noise mapping: A map-based interpolation between noise measurements with high temporal resolution

    Applied Acoustics

    (2016)
  • K.A. Berven et al.

    Dispersal in the wood frog (Rana sylvatica): Implications for genetic population structure

    Evolution

    (1990)
  • J. Bouchard et al.

    Behavioral responses of northern leopard frogs (Rana pipiens) to roads and traffic: Implications for population persistence

    Ecology and Society

    (2009)
  • H. Brumm

    The impact of environmental noise on song amplitude in a territorial bird

    Journal of Animal Ecology

    (2004)
  • J. Canicer et al.

    Geographic differences between functional groups in patterns of bird species richness in North America

    Acta Oecologica

    (2008)
  • L.A. Cartwright et al.

    Urban noise affects song structure and daily patterns of song production in Red-winged Blackbirds (Agelaius phoenicus)

    Urban Ecosystems

    (2014)
  • P. Clergeau et al.

    Bird abundance and diversity along an urban-rural gradient: A comparaive study between two cities on different continents

    The Condor

    (1998)
  • V.J. Colino-Rabanal et al.

    Herpetofauna and roads: A review

    Basic and Applied Herpetology

    (2012)
  • Commission de protection du territoire agricole

    Zone agricole

    (2009)
  • G.M. Cunnington et al.

    Mate attraction by male anurans in the presence of traffic noise

    Animal Conservation

    (2013)
  • B.M. Davidson et al.

    Natural and anthropogenic sounds reduce song performance: Insights from two embirizid species

    Behavioral Ecology

    (2017)
  • R.J. Dooling et al.

    The effects of highway noise on birds

    (2007)
  • J.A. Endler

    Some general comments on the evolution and design of animal communication systems

    Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences

    (1993)
  • A. Farina et al.

    Spatial and temporal variation of bird dawn chorus and successive acoustic morning activity in a mediterranean landscape

    Bioacoustics

    (2015)
  • A.S. Feng et al.

    Sound processing in real-world environments

  • Cited by (2)

    • Advancing avian road ecology research through systematic review

      2022, Transportation Research Part D: Transport and Environment
      Citation Excerpt :

      Fewer still managed for the occurrence of multiple sources of bias. Species richness and abundance generally increased with increasing distance from the road corridor, and these typically correlated with reduced anthropogenic influence (e.g., traffic, noise, light, infrastructure, etc.) and improved habitat condition (Lituma and Buehler, 2016; L. Desrochers and Proulx, 2017; Hennigar et al., 2019; Heggie-Gracie et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2020; Merrall and Evans, 2020; Senzaki et al., 2020). Confounding variables within field experiments are a common feature of road ecology studies but are extremely difficult to manage (Khamcha et al., 2018a; Merrall and Evans, 2020).

    View full text