The prehistory of handedness: Archaeological data and comparative ethology

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.02.012Get rights and content

Abstract

Homo sapiens sapiens displays a species wide lateralised hand preference, with 85% of individuals in all populations being right-handed for most manual actions. In contrast, no other great ape species shows such strong and consistent population level biases, indicating that extremes of both direction and strength of manual laterality (i.e., species-wide right-handedness) may have emerged after divergence from the last common ancestor. To reconstruct the hand use patterns of early hominins, laterality is assessed in prehistoric artefacts. Group right side biases are well established from the Neanderthals onward, while patchy evidence from older fossils and artefacts indicates a preponderance of right-handed individuals. Individual hand preferences and group level biases can occur in chimpanzees and other apes for skilled tool use and food processing. Comparing these findings with human ethological data on spontaneous hand use reveals that the great ape clade (including humans) probably has a common effect at the individual level, such that a person can vary from ambidextrous to completely lateralised depending on the action. However, there is currently no theoretical model to explain this result. The degree of task complexity and bimanual complementarity have been proposed as factors affecting lateralisation strength. When primatology meets palaeoanthropology, the evidence suggests species-level right-handedness may have emerged through the social transmission of increasingly complex, bimanually differentiated, tool using activities.

Introduction

Human hand use patterns can be characterised as complementary role differentiation (CRD). The CRD model of bimanual action derives from the Kinematic Chain model, which was proposed by Guiard (1987) and applied by Uomini (2006a) to the prehistoric activities that offer evidence for handedness (Steele and Uomini, 2009). In this model, one hand executes high frequency tasks (involving finer temporal and spatial resolution) while the other hand performs low frequency tasks (such as supporting an object). Rather than one hand being “dominant,” the CRD model recognises that both hands have different but equally important roles (Corbetta and Thelen, 1996). “Right-handers” are thus defined as people who prefer to adopt the high frequency role with the right hand and the low frequency role with the left hand, as shown by experiments on humans for a complementary bimanual task requiring precision versus support (Hinckley, 1996, Hinckley et al., 1997). A hand role dichotomy appears to emerge between seven and thirteen months of age (Bresson et al., 1977, Ramsay et al., 1979, Michel et al., 1985, Kimmerle et al., 1995, Michel, 1998) and is well established by age three (Ingram, 1975, Gaillard, 1996), yet its genetic determinants are still unknown (Crow, 1998, Van Agtmael et al., 2001).

Beyond the individual, Homo sapiens sapiens displays lateralised hand preference at the species level. This means that a bias to the right-handed CRD pattern is found in all human populations around the world (reviewed in Llaurens et al., 2009), with the frequency of right handed persons in any given population varying between 74% and 96% (Hardyck and Petrinovich, 1977, Porac and Coren, 1981, McManus, 1991, Connolly and Bishop, 1992, Perelle and Ehrman, 1994, Annett, 2002, Raymond and Pontier, 2004, Faurie et al., 2005). There has never been any report of a human population in which left handed individuals predominate (Llaurens et al., 2009). In contrast to the human bias, it is clear from observations of experimental and spontaneous hand actions in captive and wild subjects that the non human primates do not show a species wide consistency in hand use patterns (Colell et al., 1995, Papademetriou et al., 2005). While group level biases can occur in some populations of chimpanzees (e.g., a rightward bias at Yerkes [Hopkins et al., 2007]) and for certain manual actions in some great apes (e.g., gorillas feeding on plants [Corp and Byrne, 2004]), there is no consistent pattern across populations at the species level, since some populations are claimed to show a leftward bias (e.g., for termite fishing in Gombe's wild chimpanzees [Lonsdorf and Hopkins, 2005]).

Furthermore, these biases do not extend to all actions, nor do they reach the extreme degree of consistency seen in humans across all tasks including unimanual actions (McManus, 1985, Hopkins, 2006). As discussed below in more detail, the compilation of research led by various authors suggests that a species-wide group level manual preference across all tasks is not the norm in primates. Therefore, human handedness is unique in both its direction (rightward CRD pattern) and its strength (species wide preference), and remains to be explained in evolutionary terms.

The timing and context for the emergence of species handedness in hominins is therefore of much interest to palaeoanthropologists. Combining palaeoanthropology with primatology can help us decide whether human and nonhuman hand preference patterns are part of the same continuum, or are qualitatively different. A selection of archaeological and primatological findings for hand preference is discussed in a comparative framework, with a special focus on the population- versus species-level distinction.

Section snippets

Lateralised ancestors

Markers of hand preference in prehistory are found in material culture from the actions of lateralised tool manufacture and use that leave traces on objects, and in fossil skeletal asymmetries resulting from asymmetric use of the upper limb muscles over an individual's lifetime. These data have been extensively reviewed by Steele, 2000, Weaver et al., 2001, Steele and Uomini, 2005, Steele and Uomini, 2009, Auerbach and Ruff, 2006, Cashmore et al., 2008, and Uomini, 2008, Uomini, in press.

Comparing apes

In an ideal comparative perspective, the archaeological handedness data available for hominins would be directly compared to excavated material culture from extinct members of the extant great apes Pan, Gorilla, and Pongo. However, archaeological investigations of chimpanzee sites (Mercader et al., 2002, McGrew et al., 2006, Haslam, 2009) are just beginning, and there are virtually no post cranial pongid fossils that would be diagnostic for laterality. The most useful category of evidence

Discussion

As reviewed above, the most robust evidence for population level handedness in prehistory begins with the Neanderthals, although data from East African sites as early as 1 Ma indicate right handedness was also the norm. As in the living human hand preference surveys, there are no reports of predominant left handedness in any prehistoric data sets. These findings contrast sharply with the situation in non human primates, especially great apes, in which we find large geographical variation in

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Bill McGrew and LCHES for inviting me to participate in the fascinating conference, Palaeoanthropology Meets Primatology 1, that led to this paper. This research was presented jointly with Linda Marchant, to whom I am grateful for valuable advice on previous versions of the manuscript. Susan Antón, John Gowlett, Chris McManus, and anonymous reviewers also offered helpful critiques on a previous draft. The experiments were funded by a grant Land of Legends Lejre (formerly

References (141)

  • F. Gaillard

    Ontogenèse des latéralités. Une revue et un essai d'interprétation

    Arch. Pédiatr.

    (1996)
  • M. Hayashi

    A new notation system of object manipulation in the nesting-cup task for chimpanzees and humans

    Cortex

    (2007)
  • W.D. Hopkins et al.

    Handedness and neuroanatomical asymmetries in captive chimpanzees: a summary of 15 years of research

    Special Topics Primatol.

    (2007)
  • D. Ingram

    Motor asymmetries in young children

    Neuropsychologia

    (1975)
  • M. Kimmerle et al.

    Bimanual role-differentiated toy play during infancy

    Infant Behav. Dev.

    (1995)
  • M. Lozano et al.

    Non-masticatory uses of anterior teeth of Sima de los Huesos individuals (Sierra de Atapuerca, Spain)

    J. Hum. Evol.

    (2008)
  • L.F. Marchant et al.

    Laterality of limb function in wild chimpanzees of Gombe National Park: comprehensive study of spontaneous activities

    J. Hum. Evol.

    (1996)
  • A. Meguerditchian et al.

    Baboons communicate with their right hand

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2006)
  • G.F. Michel et al.

    Concordance of handedness between teacher and student facilitates learning manual skills

    J. Hum. Evol.

    (1985)
  • T. Nishida et al.

    Natural history of a tool-using behaviour by wild chimpanzees in feeding upon wood-boring ants

    J. Hum. Evol.

    (1982)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • M. Annett

    Handedness and Brain Asymmetry: The Right Shift Theory

    (2002)
  • M. Annett

    Tests of the right shift genetic model for two new samples of family handedness and for the data of McKeever (2000)

    Laterality

    (2007)
  • J. Apel

    Daggers Knowledge and Power. The Social Aspects of Flint-Dagger Technology in Scandinavia 2350–1500 cal BC

    (2001)
  • S.A. de Beaune

    The invention of technology. Prehistory and cognition

    Curr. Anthropol.

    (2004)
  • D. Biro et al.

    Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments

    Anim. Cogn.

    (2003)
  • C. Boesch

    Handedness in wild chimpanzees

    Int. J. Primatol.

    (1991)
  • M.P. Bryden et al.

    A new method of administering the Grooved Pegboard test: performance as a function of handedness and sex

    Brain Cogn.

    (2005)
  • R.W. Byrne et al.

    Manual dexterity in the gorilla: bimanual and digit role differentiation in a natural task

    Anim. Cogn.

    (2001)
  • R.W. Byrne et al.

    Estimating the complexity of animal behavior: how mountain gorillas eat thistles

    Behavior

    (2001)
  • R.W. Byrne et al.

    Learning by imitation: a hierarchical approach

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (1998)
  • L. Cashmore et al.

    The evolution of handedness in humans and great apes: a review and current issues

    J. Anthropol. Sci.

    (2008)
  • M. Colell et al.

    Hand preferences in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) in food-reaching and other daily activities

    Int. J. Primatol.

    (1995)
  • M.C. Corballis

    Evolution of language and laterality: a gradual descent?

    Cah. Psychol. Cogn./Curr. Psychol. Cogn.

    (1998)
  • D. Corbetta et al.

    The developmental origins of bimanual coordination: a dynamic perspective

    J. Exp. Psychol.

    (1996)
  • J.M. Cornford

    Specialized resharpening techniques and evidence of handedness

  • N. Corp et al.

    Sex difference in chimpanzee handedness

    Am. J. Phys. Anthropol.

    (2004)
  • T.J. Crow

    Sexual selection, timing and the descent of man: a theory of the genetic origins of language

    Cah. Psychol. Cogn./Curr. Psychol. Cogn.

    (1998)
  • B. Delluc et al.

    Images de la main dans notre préhistoire

    Doss. Archéol.

    (1993)
  • S.J. Evans

    The Ancient Stone Implements, Weapons and Ornaments, of Great Britain

    (1897)
  • J. Fagot et al.

    Manual laterality in nonhuman primates: a distinction between handedness and manual specialization

    Psychol. Bull.

    (1991)
  • C. Faurie et al.

    Handedness frequency over more than ten thousand years

    Proc. R. Soc. Lond.

    (2005)
  • C. Faurie et al.

    Variation in the frequency of left-handedness in traditional societies

    Curr. Anthropol.

    (2005)
  • Fischer, A., 1990. On being a pupil of a flintknapper of 11,000 years ago. In: Cziesla, E., Eickhoff, S., Arts, N.,...
  • A.W. Fletcher

    Clapping in chimpanzees: evidence of exclusive hand preference in a spontaneous, bimanual gesture

    Am. J. Primatol.

    (2006)
  • A.W. Fletcher et al.

    Laterality of hand function in naturalistic chimpanzees

    Laterality

    (2005)
  • R. Galloti et al.

    Recent activities of the Italian Archaeological Mission at Melka Kunture (Ethiopia): the Open Air Museum Project and the GIS application to the study of the Oldowan sites

  • N. Goren-Inbar et al.

    Nuts, nut cracking, and pitted stones at Gesher Benot Ya'aqov, Israel

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.

    (2002)
  • Y. Guiard

    Asymmetric division of labor in human skilled bimanual action: the kinematic chain as a model

    J. Mot. Behav.

    (1987)
  • R.G. Gunn

    Hand sizes in rock art: interpreting the measurements of hand stencils and prints

    Rock Art Res.

    (2006)
  • Cited by (128)

    • Between-task consistency, temporal stability and the role of posture in simple reach and fishing hand preference in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)

      2021, Applied Animal Behaviour Science
      Citation Excerpt :

      Understanding hand preference can create new insights into the origins of lateralization in primates (MacNeilage et al., 1987; Regaiolli et al., 2016), neural correlates of lateralization in the brain (Hopkins et al., 2015; Rogers, 2017) and its link with general aspects of behaviour (Roberts et al., 2019; Rogers, 2009, 2018). As the closest living relatives to humans, studying patterns of hand preference in chimpanzees can provide insights into the evolutionary origins of the left hemisphere specialization seen in humans (Uomini, 2009). In recent years, research has shown that hand preference can be a predictor of certain aspects of personality and cognitive style (Gordon and Rogers, 2010, 2015; Hopkins and Bennett, 1994; Rogers, 2018; Tomassetti et al., 2019).

    • Right or left? Determining the hand holding the tool from use traces

      2020, Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports
      Citation Excerpt :

      The control of fine manual actions (called “complex” motor actions) is attributed to the left hemisphere which would explain the preferential use of the right hand for this type of action (e.g. Greenfield, 1991; Johnson-Frey et al., 2005; Lewis, 2006; Schluter et al., 2001). Some authors have thus hypothesized the development of a strong manual preference in relation to the manufacture and complexity of tools (Corballis, 1987; Stock et al., 2013; Uomini and Gowlett, 2013; Uomini, 2009). Other studies show a greater effect of the nature and demands of the task as well as the type of object involved and the speed of the action (Forrester et al., 2013, 2012, 2011; Pouydebat et al., 2014; Quaresmini et al., 2014; Rogers, 2009).

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This article is part of the ‘Palaeoanthropology Meets Primatology’ Special Issue.

    View full text