Early socialization of prosocial behavior: Patterns in parents’ encouragement of toddlers’ helping in an everyday household task

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.12.010Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We examine differences in parental strategies for encouraging helping in toddlers.

  • Parents of 18-month-olds emphasized concrete, action-oriented task-based approaches.

  • Parents of 24-month-olds used more abstract, need-oriented approaches.

  • Parents regulated the attention of younger toddlers more.

  • Parents offered more social approval to older toddlers.

Abstract

Patterns in parents’ socialization of prosocial behavior in 18- and 24-month-olds (n = 46) were investigated during an everyday household chore that parents were asked to complete with their toddlers. Two socialization approaches were distinguished, one focused on specific requests for concrete actions needed to complete an immediate, concrete goal (“action-oriented”), and a second focused on the more abstract needs and emotions of the parent and the child's role as a helper (“need-oriented’). Parents were equally active at both ages in trying to elicit children's help but used different strategies with younger and older toddlers. With 18-month-olds they used more action-oriented approaches, whereas with 24-month-olds they increased their use of need-oriented approaches. They also regulated the attention of younger toddlers more, and more often socially approved older toddlers’ helping. Thus, how parents prompt, support, and encourage prosocial behavior changes over the second year from utilizing primarily concrete, goal-directed requests in the service of the immediate task, to increasingly emphasizing more abstract needs and emotions of the recipient and the child's role as a helper.

Introduction

Prosocial behavior, voluntarily acting on behalf of others out of caring and concern, is a core component of childhood social competence and healthy adjustment (Eisenberg, Fabes, & Spinrad, 2006). Although prosocial behavior is known to emerge in the second year of life (Brownell and Carriger, 1990, Brownell et al., 2009, Dunfield et al., 2011, Svetlova et al., 2010, Warneken and Tomasello, 2006, Zahn-Waxler et al., 1992), its developmental origins are not yet well understood. In a recent review of theoretical approaches to the early development of prosociality, Paulus (2014) includes, among others, “social interaction” models in which children engage in prosocial behavior to experience the pleasure generated by interacting with others, and “social normative” models in which the social environment supports and fosters prosocial behavior. The conceptualization underlying the current study aligns with both of these, emphasizing the social origins of prosocial behavior rather than the social-cognitive underpinnings, which are important as well (Brownell et al., 2013, Paulus, 2014, Vaish and Warneken, 2012).

Whereas much of the previous work on early socialization of prosocial behavior has focused on individual differences in global parenting style, such as sensitivity or responsiveness, we were interested in the process of socialization, i.e., the specific behaviors and strategies that parents use with young children to support and encourage prosociality and how these change over the second year as prosocial behavior emerges and becomes more autonomous. We thus examined how parents encouraged toddlers’ helping during a common household chore that could be undertaken together.

The current study is grounded on the premise that very early socialization of prosocial behavior trades on young children's fundamental affiliative motives. Baumeister and Leary (1995) proposed that humans have an essential need to form and maintain close social relationships; that such relationships involve affective concern and caring for one another's welfare; and that this core interpersonal motive influences much of human thought, emotion, and behavior. Bowlby (1969) similarly argued that young infants possess a basic motive for affiliation, which fosters their behavior and relationships. Despite differences in rearing, temperament, and attachment history, typically developing infants want to engage socially and emotionally with others. Investigators from a variety of perspectives have shown that such affiliative motives promote interdependence and shared goals, social emotions, and other-regarding orientation, all of which contribute to prosocial action (Carpendale et al., 2014, Dahl et al., 2011, de Waal, 2008, Hobson et al., 2009, Hrdy, 2001, Kochanska, 2002, Laible and Thompson, 2000, Rheingold, 1982, Stern, 1977, Tomasello et al., 2005, Trevarthen and Aitken, 2001).

If prosocial behavior is rooted in affiliative motives and young children are inclined to participate in joint, affiliative activity with their parents, then parents’ socialization of prosociality may both reflect and build on these motives. That is, rather than attempting to teach prosociality outright to very young children, for example by telling children to help or by reinforcing particular instances of it (Warneken and Tomasello, 2008, Warneken and Tomasello, 2013), parents may instead stimulate and encourage it by capitalizing on their children's affiliativeness to create situations where they can work together toward other-oriented goals. In an early classic study, Rheingold (1982) found that 18- to 30-month-old toddlers readily participated together with parents in household chores such as setting the table or sweeping up bits of paper, becoming involved in more than 60% of the parents’ activities. Children thereby learn by doing, becoming prosocial by participating together in parent-led prosocial activity, motivated by affiliation, before they explicitly intend to help or are aware of their role as a helper.

A small body of empirical research is consistent with this conceptualization. For example, Hammond (2011) found that mothers who included their 18- to 24-month-old toddlers in a cooperative clean-up activity after joint play had children who were later more likely to help an experimenter. In several longitudinal studies, Kochanska and her colleagues have found that when mothers and toddlers routinely engage in positive, mutually responsive affiliative activity, their children exhibit greater prosocial behavior starting in their second year and develop a stronger moral conscience (Kochanska, 2002). Finally, when 18-month-old toddlers were primed experimentally with photos depicting affiliative interactions, such as dolls hugging or holding hands, they were subsequently more likely to help an adult (Over & Carpenter, 2009). Thus, affiliative activity appears to motivate prosocial action in very young children. However, questions remain as to how parents use their young children's desire to affiliate in the service of promoting prosocial behavior.

Notably, across the childhood years, helping parents at home is related to children's prosocial behavior (Goodnow, 1988). For example, Hammond (2011) found that the more household chores 18- to 24-month-old toddlers participated in at home, the more often they helped an unfamiliar adult in the lab. Similarly, parents who expect their 2- and 3-year olds to behave prosocially in the family context, such as helping parents and participating in household chores, have children who are more socially competent later in childhood, including being more prosocial (Baumrind, 1971). Among pre-adolescents, assignment of household chores that involve other-oriented family care, such as helping in the garden or feeding pets, relates to spontaneous prosocial behavior (Grusec, Goodnow, & Cohen, 1996). We thus examine parents’ socialization strategies in the context of a household chore, laundry-hanging.

Because effective socialization practices must accommodate to the child's developmental competence, we expect that parents’ socialization of prosociality should change with the child's age and accompanying growth in emotion understanding, self-awareness, perspective taking, emotion regulation, and other capacities relevant to prosocial responding. A key developmental shift in early prosocial behavior is from children's ability to engage in “instrumental,” action-based helping behavior early in the second year to “empathic,” emotion-based helping later in the second and third years (Svetlova et al., 2010). Instrumental helping is primarily about assisting others with achieving their action-based goals, whereas empathic helping refers to efforts to alleviate another's negative affective state. For example, 14- to 18-month-old toddlers will help an adult by picking up something he has dropped or misplaced (Warneken & Tomasello, 2007), and by 24–30 months of age they can help someone who is sad or cold by giving them what they need to feel better (Svetlova et al., 2010). Corresponding to the developmental change in these forms of helping, parents would be expected to adopt a more concrete action-based, goal-oriented, and task-specific approach early in the second year that would support and encourage children's instrumental helping; later in the second year they would be expected to integrate a more indirect and abstract, need-oriented approach and a greater focus on the parent's emotions and how the child's behavior might be helpful in responding to them in support of the transition to empathic, emotion-based helping. We test this hypothesis in the current study with 18- and 24-month old toddlers.

In addition to employing strategies meant to elicit and support prosocial responding, parents are also likely to regulate their toddlers’ attention and behavior to assist children in maintaining focus on the situation and the parent's need. Particularly with young children, parents may need to be more explicit in drawing the child's attention to the task and the desired behavior, including using gestures to communicate intent and enhance task salience (Wu & Coulson, 2007). Rheingold (1982) found that parents’ directing and maintaining toddlers’ attention to household tasks was positively correlated with rates of participation in those tasks. In the current study we expected that such attention-directing behavior would be used more frequently with younger toddlers because of their more limited attentional and regulatory control.

Parents also socially reinforce young children's helping behavior, thanking and praising them for helping both in the home (Dahl, Schuck, Hung, Hsieh, & Campos, 2012) and in the laboratory (Eisenberg et al., 1992, Rheingold, 1982). Although one recent study found that material rewards reduced toddlers’ helping (Warneken & Tomasello, 2008), verbal encouragement did not (Warneken & Tomasello, 2013). In an early study of social reinforcement of prosocial behavior, parents’ praise of 1- and 2-year olds’ prosocial behavior during a play session was positively related to the children's prosociality with the parent during the session (Eisenberg et al., 1992). Social approval may be especially effective in the context of joint activity where parents may use it to scaffold mastery by informing the child that helping behavior is desirable and encouraging the child to repeat or continue such behavior. We thus expected that praise and social approval would be used by parents at both ages.

The aim of the current study was to identify how parents encourage, elicit, and maintain their toddlers’ helping behavior during a joint activity in which the parent needs assistance, and how parents’ socialization efforts change over early development as prosocial responding begins to become more abstract, need oriented, and autonomous. Eighteen- and 24-month old children were observed with their parents during an everyday household task adapted from previous research on toddlers’ participation in household routines (Rheingold, 1982) and their helping behavior in a lab task (Warneken & Tomasello, 2006). A laundry-hanging task required parents to use clothespins to fasten ‘laundry’ (cloth napkins) to a clothesline. Parents were encouraged to get their children to participate but were not told how to do so. The task was arranged to make it somewhat effortful for parents, thus providing meaningful opportunities for the parent to request help.

Parents’ naturally occurring strategies for soliciting and maintaining toddlers’ helping behavior were recorded. Use of concrete, action-oriented approaches that took the form of directives and explicit requests were distinguished from more abstract need-oriented approaches that communicated the parent's emotional state and general need for help or the value of the child's role as a helper to the parent. Parents’ efforts to focus children's attention on the task and their social approval and praise were also observed. Because individual differences in children's compliance or language skill, especially emotion vocabulary, might affect parents’ behavior above and beyond age-related contributors, these were controlled.

We hypothesized that parents would initially emphasize concrete goals and the specific actions needed to accomplish them, directing children's immediate, task-related behavior accordingly; and that with age, as children become more sensitive to others’ internal states and able to infer and act upon them, and become more aware of their role as a helper, parents would increasingly emphasize more abstract emotional states and needs, thereby conveying not only what must be done but also why.

Section snippets

Participants

Forty-six typically developing 18- and 24-month-old infants and their parents participated. Nineteen children (10 males; 9 females) were 18 months old (within one month) and twenty-seven children (15 males; 12 females) were 24 months old (within one month). The unequal n's result from the fact that children were drawn from two larger studies of early prosocial behavior with different numbers of participants. Families were recruited by mail and phone from a medium-sized US city. The majority of

Preliminary analyses

Older children scored significantly higher on verbal comprehension (24 months: M = 79.52; 18 months: M = 56.08); emotion vocabulary (24 months: M = 23.89; 18 months: M = 4.09); and willing engagement (24 months: M = 3.76; 18 months: M = 2.55) (all F's > 8.0, all p's < .01). There were also significant gender effects for willing engagement (males: M = 2.84; females: M = 3.76) and emotion vocabulary (males: M = 13.50; females: M = 23.33), with girls scoring higher in both (F's > 4.0, p's < .05). Neither verbal comprehension

Discussion

As evidence mounts that a wide range of prosocial behavior first becomes evident in the second year of life, interest in its developmental origins has increased. Recent accounts have often emphasized the phylogenetic roots of prosocial behavior and unlearned or innate structures as foundational (e.g., Davidov et al., 2013, Hamlin and Wynn, 2011, Warneken and Tomasello, 2009). As a complement to these, we have focused on potential ontogenetic processes in the young child's socialization

References (65)

  • J.K. Hamlin et al.

    Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others

    Cognitive Development

    (2011)
  • F. Warneken

    Young children proactively remedy unnoticed accidents

    Cognition

    (2013)
  • Y.C. Wu et al.

    How iconic gestures enhance communication: An ERP study

    Brain and Language

    (2007)
  • R.F. Baumeister et al.

    The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1995)
  • D. Baumrind

    Current patterns of parental authority

    Developmental Psychology

    (1971)
  • M. Beeghly

    Mothers’ internal state language to toddlers

    British Journal of Developmental Psychology

    (1986)
  • J. Bowlby

    Attachment & loss

    (1969)
  • C.A. Brownell

    Early developments in joint action

    Review of Philosophy and Psychology

    (2011)
  • C.A. Brownell

    Early development of prosocial behavior: Current perspectives

    Infancy

    (2013)
  • C. Brownell et al.

    Changes in cooperation and self-other differentiation during the second year

    Child Development

    (1990)
  • C. Brownell et al.

    Becoming a social partner with peers: Cooperation and social understanding in one- and two-year-olds

    Child Development

    (2006)
  • C.A. Brownell et al.

    Socialization of early prosocial behavior: Parents’ talk about emotions is associated with sharing and helping in toddlers

    Infancy

    (2013)
  • C.A. Brownell et al.

    To share or not to share: When do toddlers respond to another's needs?

    Infancy

    (2009)
  • E. Brummelman et al.

    That's not just beautiful—that's incredibly beautiful!” the adverse impact of inflated praise on children with low self-esteem

    Psychological science

    (2014)
  • J.S. Bruner

    Acts of meaning

    (1990)
  • J. Carpendale et al.

    On the nature of toddlers’ helping: Helping or interest in others’ activity?

    Social Development

    (2014)
  • J.I.M. Carpendale et al.

    Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children's social interaction within social interaction

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (2004)
  • A. Dahl

    The developing social context of infant helping in two U.S. samples

    Child Development

    (2015)
  • A. Dahl et al.

    Emotional action and communication in early moral development

    Emotion Review

    (2011)
  • A. Dahl et al.

    The context of early helping behavior

    (2012)
  • N. Darling et al.

    Parenting style as context: An integrative model

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1993)
  • M. Davidov et al.

    Concern for others in the first year of life: Theory, evidence, and avenues for research

    Child Development Perspectives

    (2013)
  • F.B. de Waal

    Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy

    Annual Review of Psychology

    (2008)
  • K. Dunfield et al.

    Examining the diversity of prosocial behavior: Helping, sharing, and comforting in infancy

    Infancy

    (2011)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    Prosocial development

  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    Parental values, reinforcement, and young children's prosocial behavior: A longitudinal study

    The Journal of Genetic Psychology

    (1992)
  • L. Fenson et al.

    Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories

    Applied Psycholinguistics

    (2000)
  • P.W. Garner et al.

    Mother–child conversations about emotions: Linkages to child aggression and prosocial behavior

    Social Development

    (2008)
  • J. Goodnow

    Children's household work: Its nature and functions

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1988)
  • J.E. Grusec

    Socializing concern for others in the home

    Developmental Psychology

    (1991)
  • J.E. Grusec et al.

    Household work and the development of concern for others

    Developmental Psychology

    (1996)
  • E.A. Gunderson et al.

    Parent praise to 1- to 3-year-olds predicts children's motivational frameworks 5 years later

    Child Development

    (2013)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text