Speech perception and communication ability over the telephone by Mandarin-speaking children with cochlear implants

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2013.05.015Get rights and content

Abstract

Objective

(1) To understand speech perception and communication ability through real telephone calls by Mandarin-speaking children with cochlear implants and compare them to live-voice perception, (2) to report the general condition of telephone use of this population, and (3) to investigate the factors that correlate with telephone speech perception performance.

Methods

Fifty-six children with over 4 years of implant use (aged 6.8–13.6 years, mean duration 8.0 years) took three speech perception tests administered using telephone and live voice to examine sentence, monosyllabic-word and Mandarin tone perception. The children also filled out a questionnaire survey investigating everyday telephone use. Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used to compare the scores between live-voice and telephone tests, and Pearson's test to examine the correlation between them.

Results

The mean scores were 86.4%, 69.8% and 70.5% respectively for sentence, word and tone recognition over the telephone. The corresponding live-voice mean scores were 94.3%, 84.0% and 70.8%. Wilcoxon signed-rank test showed the sentence and word scores were significantly different between telephone and live voice test, while the tone recognition scores were not, indicating tone perception was less worsened by telephone transmission than words and sentences. Spearman's test showed that chronological age and duration of implant use were weakly correlated with the perception test scores. The questionnaire survey showed 78% of the children could initiate phone calls and 59% could use the telephone 2 years after implantation.

Conclusion

Implanted children are potentially capable of using the telephone 2 years after implantation, and communication ability over the telephone becomes satisfactory 4 years after implantation.

Introduction

The benefits of cochlear implantation (CI) in speech perception, communication skills as well as language and reading developments have been adequately proved. However, its effects in some difficult tasks such as hearing in noisy environments, telephone use and music perception are less well defined.

The understanding of telephone speech by CI recipients has been investigated by many researchers using different methods, and they have demonstrated that CI users are capable of some degree of telephone communication [1], [2]. Nevertheless, Ito et al. [3] and Milchard and Cullington [4] pointed out that telephone speech was perceived worse by the patients than live voice. Many of the previous studies used simulated telephone speech with limited bandwidth (300–3200 Hz) to test CI users’ telephone speech perception and compared the results with natural broadband speech [4], [5], [6]. Horng et al. [6] tested 15 children with cochlear implants to examine their perception of vowels, consonants, voice gender and Mandarin tones in limited-bandwidth speech, and the results showed that the phonemes (vowels and consonants) and voice gender identification were poorer with the simulated telephone speech than with the broadband one, while the tone recognition did not differ between two types of speech. Fu and Galvin [5] also used simulated band-limiting speech to test ten adult CI users on their capability of perceiving vowels, consonants and sentences over the telephone. They found that identification of consonants and sentences differed significantly between band-limiting and broadband speech, whereas that of vowels did not. These studies all suggest that restricted bandwidth of telephone speech may result in a loss of high-frequency cues, which leads to poorer comprehension.

Questionnaire surveys (telephone profile) on the frequency and experiences of the patients’ telephone use in everyday life are often administered as another way to investigate CI users’ capability of telephone communication. Earlier studies showed that, generally speaking, 56–75% of implanted adults were capable of using landline telephones and 45–75% are able to use cellular phones to communicate with other people [2], [7], [8], [9], [10], [11]. Cray et al.’s [2] study indicated that, of all his patients who were considered telephone users, most were younger and had a longer duration of implantation than non-telephone users. His study showed that 95% of them were able to converse over the telephone with familiar people and 85% with strangers.

Most of the studies on telephone use, however, were conducted on adult populations or even on postlingually deafened adults. Not much data available on prelingually deafened pediatric CI users could be found. Carmel et al. [12] was one of the few studies on the pediatric populations. They used questionnaires and found that 88% of the children with CIs were capable of initiating telephone calls, but only 38% of them had practiced talking on the phone. Tait et al. [13] used telephone profile to monitor the use of telephone of 150 children with up to 5-year experience after implantation. Their results showed a significant progress over time and were highly correlated with tests of speech perception. Nevertheless, Tait et al. [13] pointed out that telephone speech was still more difficult than live speech with the fact that, by three or 4 years after implantation, most children were able to do connected discourse tracking and closed sentence sets in live speech, while on telephone most of them were only capable of identifying single words. The difficulties of telephone speech, as Tait et al. [13] suggested, may come from its lower speech quality due to limited bandwidth as well as from lack of experience.

Mandarin Chinese is a tonal language. Each monosyllable carries one of the four tones or pitch contours that delivers different lexical meanings. The fundamental frequency (F0) patterns during voiced speech characterize the four tones. It has been well known that most of the CI speech processing strategies fail to support tone recognition with sufficient spectral or temporal cues. Fu et al. [14] and Wei et al. [15] showed in their studies that Mandarin-speaking CI patients recognized only 50–70% of Chinese tones with the latest CI technology because F0 and its harmonics were not clearly cued in electric hearing. Earlier research has suggested that Mandarin-speaking CI users chiefly used amplitude contour and periodicity information as cues to decode Mandarin tones [14], [16], [17]. Horng et al. [6] further indicated that these cues may be largely preserved in telephone speech since Mandarin tone recognition was not significantly affected by telephone speech simulation even though the high-frequency information was deprived.

Past literature have shown that, in general, both adult and pediatric CI users are capable of using the telephone to some degree. However, these studies used either a simulated setting or a questionnaire survey to investigate the telephone use by implanted populations. There is hardly any study probing the problem of real telephone use. The present study used a cohort of CI children to examine their perceptual performance on actual telephone speech. By extending the study of Horng et al. [6] which tested phonemes and tones, this study tested the perception of tones, monosyllabic words and sentences, intending a telephone test closer to real telephone use. A questionnaire survey was also conducted as supportive data that helped us have a better view on everyday telephone use by Mandarin-speaking implanted children. The specific aim of this study is (1) to understand recipients of CIs’ speech perception and Mandarin tone recognition over the telephone and compare it with their performance on the same tests using live voice, (2) to report the general condition of telephone use by Mandarin-speaking children with over 4 years of implant experience (mean duration of implant use = 8.0 years), and (3) to investigate the predictive factors that correlate with the perception on the telephone and live-voice speech, such as their age at the detection of hearing loss, age at implantation, chronological age, duration of implant use, duration of post-operative rehabilitation and pre-operative residual hearing.

Section snippets

Participants

Fifty-six children (27 boys and 29 girls) participated in this study. The inclusion criteria were that (1) the duration of implant use was more than 4 years, (2) the subjects did not have other neurological and/or developmental problems or multiple handicaps and (3) all subjects used oral communication. A total of 61 children who received implantation in our hospital met the criteria, yet five of them refused to do the tests, resulting in 56 subjects participating in this study. They were

Results

The mean percentages of correct responses (hereafter % correct) of each test are shown in Table 1. Wilcoxon signed-rank test indicated that the scores of the sentence test and the PB word test were significantly lower in telephone test than in live voice test (p < .001). The participants had a rather high % correct on these three tests delivered in live voice. Their mean % correct of the sentence test was near maximum (94.25%), and the lowest mean % correct obtained from the PB word test (84%)

Discussion

It has long been an issue for many researchers to investigate the effects of the limited bandwidth of telephone speech on CI users’ perception. Many of the previous studies used simulated telephone speech and mostly were done on adults. We believe that the present study is the first to deal with speech perception of real telephone use by Mandarin-speaking prelingually deafened children with CIs.

This study obtained the results similar to Horng et al. [6] in terms of tone recognition. In Horng et

Conclusion

The results suggest that most of the children with CIs are able to start using the telephone after 2 years of implant use, and their ability to communicate on the telephone was satisfactorily sufficient after 4 years of implant use. This indicates that children with CIs have the potential to answer phone calls. As long as the children were familiar with telephone use, they are able to adequately finish a telephone call even without special training. The recognition scores of the Mandarin tones

References (28)

  • J. Ito et al.

    Hearing ability by telephone of patients with cochlear implants

    Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg.

    (1999)
  • C.G. Wei et al.

    Mandarin tone recognition in cochlear-implant subjects

    Hear. Res.

    (2004)
  • N.L. Cohen et al.

    Telephone speech comprehension with use of the nucleus cochlear implant

    Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. Suppl.

    (1989)
  • J.W. Cray et al.

    An investigation of telephone use among cochlear implant recipients

    Am. J. Audiol.

    (2004)
  • A.J. Milchard et al.

    An investigation into the effect of limiting the frequency bandwidth of speech on speech recognition in adult cochlear implant users

    Int. J. Audiol.

    (2004)
  • Q.J. Fu et al.

    Recognition of simulated telephone speech by cochlear implant users

    Am. J. Audiol.

    (2006)
  • M.J. Horng et al.

    Telephone speech perception by Mandarin-speaking cochlear implantees

    Ear Hear.

    (2007)
  • J.S. Adams et al.

    Telephone use and understanding in patients with cochlear implants

    Ear. Nose. Throat J.

    (2004)
  • I. Anderson et al.

    Telephone use: what benefit do cochlear implant users receive?

    Int. J. Audiol.

    (2006)
  • A. Castro et al.

    Comparison of different mobile telephones in Combi40+ users

    Acta Otolaryngol.

    (2006)
  • G.H. Chee et al.

    Benefits of cochlear implantation in early-deafened adults: the Toronto experience

    J. Otolaryngol.

    (2004)
  • L. Lassaletta et al.

    Quality of life in postlingually deaf patients following cochlear implantation

    Eur. Arch. Otorhinolaryngol.

    (2006)
  • E. Carmel et al.

    Telephone use among cochlear implanted children

    Acta Otolaryngol.

    (2011)
  • M. Tait et al.

    Use of the telephone in prelingually deaf children with a multichannel cochlear implant

    Otol. Neurotol.

    (2001)
  • Cited by (9)

    • Technical aids for speech understanding in cochlear implanted adults using cell-phones

      2016, Annales Francaises d'Oto-Rhino-Laryngologie et de Pathologie Cervico-Faciale
    • Technical aids for speech understanding in cochlear implanted adults using cell-phones

      2016, European Annals of Otorhinolaryngology, Head and Neck Diseases
      Citation Excerpt :

      Telephone use is low in cochlear implant bearers. In the 2012 CISIC survey [6], 59% of CI bearers used the telephone with the implant; exactly the same figure was reported in a Chinese pediatric population 2 years after implantation [7], without details on the quality of speech perception. Various listening aids have been developed in recent years, significantly improving understanding on the phone in both silence and noise.

    • Auditory performance and speech intelligibility of Mandarin-speaking children implanted before age 5

      2014, International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Beadle et al. [3] also noted that after 10 years of implant use, 60% out of 30 subjects were able to use the telephone. Our previous study on telephone speech [22] indicated that 46% of the children were able to complete a phone call by themselves after more than four years of implant use (mean = 8 years). Both studies suggest that CI children require more practice with telephone use to reach the highest CAP level.

    • Loudness Contour Can Influence Mandarin Tone Recognition: Vocoder Simulation and Cochlear Implants

      2017, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text