Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 136, March 2015, Pages 186-195
Cognition

What is the link between synaesthesia and sound symbolism?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.013Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We tested synaesthetes’ and controls’ ability to determine foreign word meanings.

  • All participants correctly identified some word meanings at above chance rates.

  • Grapheme-colour synaesthetes performed significantly better than nonsynaesthetes.

  • The same cross-sensory integration driving synaesthesia may underlie sound symbolism.

  • Synaesthesia endows/co-occurs with enhanced multisensory skills in unrelated domains.

Abstract

Sound symbolism is a property of certain words which have a direct link between their phonological form and their semantic meaning. In certain instances, sound symbolism can allow non-native speakers to understand the meanings of etymologically unfamiliar foreign words, although the mechanisms driving this are not well understood. We examined whether sound symbolism might be mediated by the same types of cross-modal processes that typify synaesthetic experiences. Synaesthesia is an inherited condition in which sensory or cognitive stimuli (e.g., sounds, words) cause additional, unusual cross-modal percepts (e.g., sounds trigger colours, words trigger tastes). Synaesthesia may be an exaggeration of normal cross-modal processing, and if so, there may be a link between synaesthesia and the type of cross-modality inherent in sound symbolism. To test this we predicted that synaesthetes would have superior understanding of unfamiliar (sound symbolic) foreign words. In our study, 19 grapheme-colour synaesthetes and 57 non-synaesthete controls were presented with 400 adjectives from 10 unfamiliar languages and were asked to guess the meaning of each word in a two-alternative forced-choice task. Both groups showed superior understanding compared to chance levels, but synaesthetes significantly outperformed controls. This heightened ability suggests that sound symbolism may rely on the types of cross-modal integration that drive synaesthetes’ unusual experiences. It also suggests that synaesthesia endows or co-occurs with heightened multi-modal skills, and that this can arise in domains unrelated to the specific form of synaesthesia.

Introduction

Sound symbolism is a property of certain words which have a direct link between their phonological form and their semantic meaning. There is a rich history of research into sound symbolism, starting perhaps with Köhler (1929), who found that participants shared preferences for the naming of novel objects: they reliably matched nonwords such as baluma to rounded shapes, and nonwords such as takete to angular shapes. This finding has been extended by other authors, who suggest this shows a non-arbitrary relationship between sound and meaning: that there is something ‘rounded’ about the sounds comprising baluma and something ‘angular’ about takete (Davis, 1961, Maurer et al., 2006, Ramachandran and Hubbard, 2001). Sound symbolism also occurs in the real words of natural languages. English speakers are able to guess the meanings of foreign dimensional adjectives (e.g., meaning: big/small, round/pointy, fast/slow, etc.) at above-chance levels, for words in Albanian, Dutch, Gujarati, Indonesian, Korean, Mandarin, Romanian, Tamil, Turkish, Yoruba, Chinese, Czech, Hindi, Japanese, and Tahitian (Brown et al., 1955, DeFife et al., in preparation, Klank et al., 1971, Kunihira, 1971). This again suggests some inherent clues to meaning in the form of those words. Berlin (1994) demonstrated the presence of sound symbolism beyond dimensional adjectives, in a study investigating bird and fish names in the Peruvian language Huambisa; native English speakers correctly categorised bird names at rates significantly higher than chance (Berlin, 1994). An acoustic analysis of these words revealed that high frequency segments characterised bird names while low frequency segments characterised fish names. This demonstrates that the Huambisa language contains sound symbolic phonological patterns to distinguish bird and fish names, and furthermore, that native English speakers are capable of decoding these patterns. Farmer, Christiansen, and Monaghan (2006) also demonstrated the presence of sound symbolism within English, finding that English nouns and verbs have category-typical phonological properties and, furthermore, that listeners are sensitive to these properties during on-line processing tasks. The cross-linguistic presence of sound-to-meaning mappings, and the ability to deduce sound-to-meaning mappings in other languages, suggests that vocabulary is not arbitrarily assigned (or processed) and that it may be guided by shared cross-modal mechanisms. Nonetheless, the exact nature of these mechanisms is not well understood.

In the present study, we sought a better understanding of sound symbolism by comparison with a case of extreme cross-modal processing known as synaesthesia. For people with synaesthesia, sensory or cognitive stimuli (e.g., written words) induce the experience of unusual additional percepts, either in the same modality (e.g., the colour red) or in a different modality (e.g., the taste of oranges). Grapheme-colour synaesthetes, for example, experience colours triggered by reading, hearing, saying or thinking about graphemes (e.g., a = red; e.g., Simner, Glover, & Mowat, 2006). The condition has a genetic basis (Asher et al., 2009, Tomson et al., 2011) and is typified by anatomical differences including altered white-matter coherence (e.g., Rouw & Scholte, 2007) and grey matter volume (Weiss & Fink, 2009). Synaesthesia is thought to arise from either excess cortical connections or disinhibition of existing circuits (or both; see Bargary & Mitchell, 2008, for review). In behavioural terms, synaesthesia causes a type of unusual ‘cross-talk’ between modalities, and in the present study we ask whether a comparable type of cross-talk might also underlie normal linguistic sound symbolism.

It has been suggested that synaesthesia represents an enhancement or explicit manifestation of latent implicit cross-modal associations found in the general population (see below). Since sound symbolism is a case of cross-modal association, the enhanced cross-modal state of synaesthetes might afford synaesthetes superior abilities in sound symbolic tasks. In our study we asked synaesthetes and controls to guess the meanings of foreign words in languages they do not speak. If synaesthetes show superior understanding of sound symbolic meanings this would be the first explicit link between synaesthetic and sound symbolic cognition, and would provide a novel way to frame this relatively poorly understood area of language processing. Such a finding would also shed light on the unusual condition of synaesthesia, per se, by showing that synaesthetes might be unusually skilled in cross-modal tasks entirely unrelated to their synaesthesia.

A possible link between synaesthetic and ‘normal’ processing is already motivated by prior studies. Although synaesthetic experiences are superficially idiosyncratic from one synaesthete to the next (e.g., the letter a might be red for one synaesthete but green for another), many types of synaesthesia often reflect patterns found intuitively in the general population (see Simner, 2013 for review). Sound-colour synaesthetes, for example, tend to ‘see’ higher pitch sounds as lighter colours, and nonsynaesthetes tend to favour this same mapping by intuition, in forced-choice cross-sensory association tasks (Marks, 1974, Ward et al., 2006). Many forms of synaesthesia follow this same general principle of reflecting nonsynaesthetes’ implicit associations (e.g., Cytowic and Wood, 1982, Marks, 1974, Marks, 1987, Simner, 2012, Simner et al., 2005, Smilek et al., 2007, Ward et al., 2006). These common patterns across synaesthetes and nonsynaesthetes suggest that synaesthesia might be an exaggeration or heightened awareness of cross-modal associations present in the general population. If synaesthesia is a superior manifestation of normal cross-modality, this may allow synaesthetes to perform better than nonsynaesthetes in a range of cross- modal tasks, including perhaps, those relating to sound symbolism.

Evidence for synaesthetes’ superior performance in other areas of cross-modality has been demonstrated by Brang, Williams, and Ramachandran (2011). They showed that grapheme-colour synaesthetes have a heightened sensitivity to cross-modal associations in a double-flash illusion task: participants reported the number of visual flashes perceived (1 or 2) in conditions where the flashes were accompanied by either the same number of auditory beeps, or a different number. Synaesthetes were significantly less accurate in the incongruent condition (1 flash, 2 beeps) compared to nonsynaesthetes, suggesting they more strongly integrated the visual and auditory signals. In a second task, synaesthetes benefited more from bimodal stimuli than nonsynaesthetes when detecting both unimodal (auditory beep or visual flash) or bimodal stimuli. Since grapheme-colour synaesthetes do not experience synaesthesia for flashes or beeps, these findings show that their cross-modal skills extend to stimuli beyond those involved in their specific type of synaesthesia (Brang et al., 2011; but see Neufeld, Sinke, Zedler, Emrich, & Szycik, 2012, for evidence that older synaesthetes may lose this advantage). Although synaesthetes have increased multimodal integration, it is not known if this potential advantage could also be found in ‘higher level’ cognitive cross-modal processing, such as the language processing of sound symbolism.

To determine whether synaesthetes have heightened awareness of sound symbolism compared to nonsynaesthetes, we employed a two-alternative forced-choice task. For each trial, participants listened to a foreign word (e.g., aravam: Tamil) and chose its meaning from two English antonyms (e.g., loud or quiet). We predicted that synaesthetes would have higher accuracy in this task than nonsynaesthetes. To ensure that any difference in performance across groups was not due to a general superior cognitive ability or to any increased motivation on the part of our synaesthetes (see Gheri, Chopping, & Morgan, 2008), we also tested synaesthetes on a second task (the vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised, WAIS-R) where we predicted no difference between groups. It is particularly important to check for motivational biases,1 given that synaesthetes are recruited as a special population. We predict that synaesthetes will out-perform nonsynaesthetes in the sound symbolism language task but not in the vocabulary task. Finally, we also utilise this study to gain other novel information about the phenomenon of sound symbolism. Given adults’ sensitivity to environmental statistics, which is well documented in the statistical learning literature (e.g., Pelucchi et al., 2009, Fiser and Aslin, 2001), we ask whether the sound-meaning correspondences in our stimuli are learned during the experiment. Furthermore, we hypothesise that if learning of sound-meaning correspondences does occur during the experiment, synaesthetes may be faster to pick up on these cross-modal statistics than nonsynaesthetes (Gross, Neargarder, Caldwell-Harris, & Cronin-Golomb, 2011).

Section snippets

Participants

Nineteen native English-speaking grapheme-colour synaesthetes (mean age = 42.74, SD = 15.95, 3 male) were recruited from the Sussex-Edinburgh database of Synaesthete Participants and compensated £10.00 for participation. Fifty-seven native English-speaking nonsynaesthetes were recruited as age-matched controls (three per synaesthete). Controls were tested in Rochester, NY (n = 18) and Edinburgh, UK (n = 39).

Results

Before performing our analysis, we noted that our foreign words contained 45 cognates of English. Cognates were defined as words whose meaning could be guessed based on knowledge of English alone (e.g., larg = “big”; Romanian). Superior performance in our task based on etymological similarity is not necessarily contradictory to the idea of sound symbolism since these etymological similarities may have been preserved throughout language evolution due to the benefit that sound symbolism yields for

Discussion

Our investigation has looked at sound symbolism – a linguistic property in which phonological forms map to semantic meanings, and which can sometimes allow non-native speakers to understand unfamiliar foreign words. Our aim was to shed light on a relatively poorly understood area of language by showing that sound symbolism may arise from latent cross-modal mechanisms of a type found more extremely in synaesthetes. Our data show that synaesthetes had superior sensitivities to linguistic

Acknowledgements

We sincerely thank Lauren DeFife, Lynne Nygaard, and Laura Namy for graciously sharing their stimuli with us. This work was supported in part by a US-UK Fulbright Grant to author KB and NIH Grant HD-037082. We would like to thank Jessica Briggs, Tale Gundersen, Alex Henniker-Heaton, Millie Marshall, Emma McBride, Malcolm Peacock and Natalie Wills for their help in data collection.

References (58)

  • S. Kunihira

    Effects of expressive voice on phonetic symbolism

    Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior

    (1971)
  • G.L. Murphy et al.

    The conceptual basis of antonymy and synonymy in adjectives

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (1993)
  • J. Neufeld et al.

    Reduced audio-visual integration in synaestehtes indicated by the double-flash illusion

    Brain Research

    (2012)
  • L.C. Nygaard et al.

    Sound to meaning correspondences facilitate word learning

    Cognition

    (2009)
  • K.P. Revill et al.

    Cross-linguistic sound symbolism and crossmodal correspondence: Evidence from fMRI and DTI

    Brain and Language

    (2014)
  • H. Shintel et al.

    Analog acoustic expression in speech communication

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (2006)
  • W.K. Simmons et al.

    A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color

    Neuropsychologia

    (2007)
  • J. Simner

    Beyond perception: Synaesthesia as a psycholinguistic phenomenon

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2007)
  • J. Simner et al.

    Linguistic determinants of word colouring in grapheme-colour synaesthesia

    Cortex

    (2006)
  • S.N. Tomson et al.

    The genetics of colored sequence synaesthesia: Suggestive evidence of linkage to 16q and genetic heterogeneity for the condition

    Behavioral Brain Research

    (2011)
  • J. Ward et al.

    Sound-colour synaesthesia: To what extent does it use cross-modal mechanisms common to us all?

    Cortex

    (2006)
  • B. Berlin

    Evidence for pervasive synaesthetic sound symbolism in ethnozoological nomenclature

  • J. Blaha et al.

    Hierarchical factor structure of the wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1982)
  • D. Brang et al.

    Grapheme-color synaesthetes show enhanced crossmodal processing between auditory and visual modalities

    Cortex

    (2011)
  • R.W. Brown et al.

    Phonetic symbolism in natural languages

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (1955)
  • C. Cuskley et al.

    Synaesthesia, cross-modality and language evolution

  • R. Davis

    The fitness of names to drawings: A crosscultural study in Tanganyika

    British Journal of Psychology

    (1961)
  • DeFife, L., Nygaard, L.C., & Namy, L.L. (in preparation). Cross-linguistic consistency and within-language variability...
  • A. Dovern et al.

    Intrinsic network connectivity reflects consistency of synaesthetic experiences

    The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

    (2012)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text