Elsevier

Brain Research

Volume 1263, 31 March 2009, Pages 69-77
Brain Research

Research Report
Neuroanatomical correlates of visual field bias: A sensitive system for detecting potential threats?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009.01.051Get rights and content

Abstract

Preferred pathways of emotional processing were investigated in an fMRI study where fearful and neutral faces were presented in the left, right or central visual field. Seventeen male volunteers were imaged at 3.0 T using GE-EPI. Spatial preprocessing and statistical analyses were performed using SPM2. A preference for peripheral fearful and central neutral faces was found in frontal and temporal areas, temporal pole, putamen, and ACC. A subset showed a significant preference for fearful faces in the right hemifield. These results seem to suggest a sensitive network for preferential processing of potential threat signals.

Introduction

The concept of cerebral lateralization emerged in the second half of the 19th century, triggered by Broca's discoveries of lateralized language functions. A link between the right hemisphere and emotional processing was established as early as in the beginning of the 20th century by Mills, 1912a, Mills, 1912b. Based on neuropsychological observations in patients with unilateral brain lesions, the right hemisphere model was developed which states that the right hemisphere is dominant in emotional processing, regardless of valence (Borod et al., 1983). However, several observations, in particular on healthy subjects, did not seem to support this model. Therefore, more recent hypotheses posit that hemispheric specialisation of emotion depends on valence (Ehrlichman, 1987), on associated approach or withdrawal behaviour (Davidson et al., 1990), or reflects behavioural activation and inhibition systems (Gray and McNaughton, 2000). For a recent review of these theories see Demaree et al. (2005).

Generally, a visual-field bias in the judgment of facial expressions (Adolphs, 2001, Asthana and Mandal, 2001, Jansari, 2000) or a bias in infant holding (Vauclair and Donnot, 2005) is interpreted in terms of the hemispheric dominance theory. Images presented in the left or right visual field are assumed to be preferentially processed by the right or left hemisphere, respectively. The finding of Jansari et al. (2000) that the perceptual discrimination of emotional faces from neutral faces was better if positively valenced faces were presented to the viewer's right and negatively valenced faces were presented to the viewer's left was thus interpreted as a preference of the right and left hemisphere for negatively and positively valenced emotions, respectively. Until the 1970s, functional hemispheric specialisation was thought to be a specific human trait. However, since then cerebral asymmetry has also been found in a number of vertebrates. In animals with laterally placed eyes, e.g. birds, toads and fish, significant asymmetries in eye use have been detected, and a study in chick embryos even revealed the genetic, hormonal and experiential factors that interact to determine the lateralized organization of visual functions (see Vallortigara and Rogers, 2005 for review). Thus, it seems that hemispheric specialisation is widespread in animals and may have emerged early in evolution.

This brings up the question about the purpose of functional hemispheric specialisation of the brain. Obviously, there should be clear disadvantages for creatures with significant perceptual asymmetries because the physical world doesn't show any preference for left and right, and enemies as well as prey might approach from any side (Vallortigara and Rogers, 2005). However, possibly clear-cut advantages arise from functional specialisation. Firstly, neural capacity should be increased because specialisation of one hemisphere for a particular function leaves the other hemisphere free for different tasks. Secondly, the dominance of one hemisphere would be a convenient way of preventing simultaneous initiation of incompatible responses in organisms with laterally placed eyes (Vallortigara and Rogers, 2005). In the light of these hypothetical advantages and the above-mentioned findings in a variety of animals, it seems reasonable to assume that humans also show perceptual asymmetries, even though those were only detected in highly artificial experimental situations (i.e., tachistoscopic viewing or dichotic listening) and do not seem to play a direct role in everyday live.

The advent of neuroimaging techniques like positron emission tomography (PET), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electrophysiological methods like electro encephalography (EEG) and magnetic encephalography (MEG) allows to investigate these hypotheses in more detail in healthy human subjects. While the lateralization was found to be quite strict at the primary sensory level where input from the right visual field is processed in the left visual cortex and vice versa, matters are less clear for more complex cognitive functions, e.g. for language processing which was generally supposed to be a left hemispheric function, at least in right-handers (Lindell, 2006). Thus, not surprisingly, recent meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies of emotion conclude that the concept of lateralization of emotional processing cannot be supported on a hemispheric level, suggesting that functional specialisation of brain regions involved in emotional processing is rather more complex and region-specific (Murphy, 2003, Phan, 2002, Wager, 2003) than predicted by previous theories based on lesion studies and neuropsychological observations. However, functional specialisation does not necessarily require lateralization to one hemisphere. In our opinion, the observed visual field biases (Adolphs, 2001, Asthana and Mandal, 2001, Jansari, 2000, Vauclair and Donnot, 2005) suggest that there are preferred pathways for emotional contents which may also depend on the side of input.

In order to investigate the hypothesized preferred pathways in more detail we performed an fMRI study, presenting fearful and neutral faces to the left, right or central visual field. Our hypothesis is that a visual field bias in the judgment of facial expressions should be reflected in the neural substrates of emotional processing. We expected to find specific neuronal networks for emotional processing depending on the side of input and emotional content.

Section snippets

Main effects

The main effects of visual activation for face stimuli presented in the left, central and right visual field were strongly lateralized to contralateral early visual brain areas. For the main effect of fearful faces > neutral faces (contrast − 1 − 1 − 1 1 1 1 0 0 0), significant activation was only found in a small cluster (12 voxels) in the area of the left superior temporal pole and amygdala (− 33 0 − 24, t = 4.45). The reversed contrast did not yield any significant activation.

Interaction of facial expression and site of image presentation (− 1 2 − 1 1 − 2 1 0 0 0)

A network of brain

Discussion

In general, face perception is supposed to be mediated by a distributed cortical network comprising the inferior occipital gyrus, fusiform gyrus, superior temporal sulcus, hippocampus, amygdala, inferior frontal gyrus and orbitofrontal cortex (Haxby, 2000, Ishai, 2005). In the present study we intended to reveal potential neuronal substrates of visual field bias. In our opinion, the detected set of brain regions showing stronger activation for peripherally presented fearful and centrally

Subjects

The study presented here was approved by the local ethics committee and performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. Because of probable gender differences in brain lateralization and evidence for a more pronounced hemispheric asymmetry in emotional processing in men (Proverbio et al., 2006), twenty healthy male volunteers were recruited for this study. All subjects gave written informed consent before participating in the study. Three of the

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all volunteers who participated in this study. This study was supported by the Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (Brain Imaging Center Frankfurt, DLR 01GO0203).

References (57)

  • HaxbyJ.V.

    The distributed human neural system for face perception

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2000)
  • IshaiA.

    Face perception is mediated by a distributed cortical network

    Brain Res. Bull.

    (2005)
  • Kesler-WestM.L.

    Neural substrates of facial emotion processing using fMRI

    Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2001)
  • MitchellJ.P.

    General and specific contributions of the medial prefrontal cortex to knowledge about mental states

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • NomuraM.

    Frontal lobe networks for effective processing of ambiguously expressed emotions in humans

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2003)
  • OldfieldR.C.

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia.

    (1971)
  • PhanK.L.

    Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • VauclairJ. et al.

    Infant holding biases and their relations to hemispheric specializations for perceiving facial emotions

    Neuropsychologia

    (2005)
  • VuilleumierP.

    Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study

    Neuron

    (2001)
  • WagerT.D.

    Valence, gender, and lateralization of functional brain anatomy in emotion: a meta-analysis of findings from neuroimaging

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • WeiskopfN.

    Optimal EPI parameters for reduction of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses: a whole-brain analysis at 3 T and 1.5 T

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • WorsleyK.J. et al.

    Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited-again

    NeuroImage

    (1995)
  • WrightP. et al.

    Neutral faces activate the amygdala during identity matching

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • AdolphsR.

    Hemispheric perception of emotional valence from facial expressions

    Neuropsychology

    (2001)
  • AmaralD.G. et al.

    Amygdalo-cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca fascicularis)

    J. Comp. Neurol.

    (1984)
  • AmodioD.M. et al.

    Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2006)
  • AndersonA.K.

    Neural correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial signals

    J. Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • AshburnerJ. et al.

    Nonlinear spatial normalization using basis functions

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (1999)
  • Cited by (9)

    • Angiotensin Regulation of Amygdala Response to Threat in High-Trait-Anxiety Individuals

      2018, Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging
      Citation Excerpt :

      Increased activation in these areas can be seen during extinction learning and recall (58–61). Similarly, putamen response has been associated with enhanced threat processing (62–64), and reduced activity has been seen to correlate with directing attention away from threat stimuli as a form of avoidance (65). Avoidance of threat stimuli—attentional, cognitive–emotional, or physical—is thought to play a crucial role in maintaining anxiety because it prevents the patient from experiencing the absence of the anticipated catastrophe.

    • The role of the amygdala and the basal ganglia in visual processing of central vs. peripheral emotional content

      2013, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      In our study, we used an intermediate visual angle, 7.71°, which is more close to the study reporting a central bias. Second, magnification factors (used in Morawetz et al., 2011, 2010; Liu & Ioannides, 2010; Preibisch et al., 2009 but see Bayle et al., 2009) may be an issue. We addressed this issue by using control non-face stimuli that were scale matched to the face stimuli.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text