High levels of psychopathic traits increase the risk of transferring reactive aggression to innocent people after provocation: Evidence from an ERP study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107891Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Participants with different levels of psychopathic traits completed a modified TAP.

  • Psychopathic traits enhanced aggressive behavior to the innocents.

  • A more negative N2 and a smaller P3 reflected more aggression to the innocents.

Abstract

This study investigated the neuropsychological underpinnings of reactive aggression toward innocent people in a student population with different levels of psychopathic traits. While recording event-related potentials, participants (divided into high/low psychopathic [HP/LP] traits groups) competed against two fictitious opponents in a modified Taylor Aggression Paradigm. We found that the HP group compared to the LP group selected more often high-intensity punishment for the second innocent opponent after being provoked by the first opponent. Further, a more negative N2 and a smaller P3 was found in the HP group while punishing the innocents—reflecting a tendency on antisocial-aggressive behavior. Finally, both groups showed a more negative FRN for losing than winning trials when seeing the outcome of the game. Our results suggest that high psychopathic traits increase the risk of transferring provoked aggression to innocent people—offering a psychophysiological perspective for explaining and predicting aggression against the innocents in social interactions.

Introduction

Psychopathy is an intriguing concept for both researchers and laypeople. At the same time, however, it is one of the most disturbing and hardly treatable mental disorders in the world (Vien, 2006). Psychopathy is associated with a collection of affective, interpersonal, and behavioral features, including shallow affect, lack of remorse or guilt, callousness, grandiosity, pathological lying, manipulation, irresponsibility, impulsivity, poor self-control, and sensation seeking (Cleckley, 1951; Hare, 1980; Harpur, Hare, & Hakstian, 1989). About 15∼20% of the forensic population and about 1% of the general population (Glenn, Kurzban, & Raine, 2011; Hare, 1991) meets the diagnostic criteria for psychopathy. However, it has been recognized that psychopathic features also exist as personality traits (Edens, Marcus, Lilienfeld, & Poythress, 2006; Glenn et al., 2011; Guay, Ruscio, Knight, & Hare, 2007). High levels of psychopathic traits can be readily found in normal-functioning non-forensic samples without fulfilling the criteria of psychopathy (Benning, Patrick, Hicks, Blonigen, & Krueger, 2003; Ross, Benning, Patrick, Thompson, & Thurston, 2009).

Psychopathic traits are one of the most widely recognized risk factors for aggressive behavior (Guerra & White, 2017). Aggression can be divided into proactive aggression (PA; or instrumental aggression) and reactive aggression (RA; or impulsive aggression) based on the functions of behavior (Poulin & Boivin, 2000; Preston, Anestis, Watts, & Lilienfeld, 2018). Proactive aggression has been described as a goal-directed, forethoughtful and organized act without previous provocation. This form of aggressive behavior functions as a means to gain resources or attains domination and intimidation (Berkowitz, 1983; Dodge & Coie, 1987). In contrast, reactive aggression refers to an impulsive and emotion-driven act that involves high affective-physiological arousal. This form of aggressive behavior occurs as a hostile response to interpersonal provocation or threat (Chase, O’Leary, & Heyman, 2001; Dodge & Coie, 1987; Poulin & Boivin, 2000).

Extensive research exists on the relationship between psychopathy and proactive aggression—yielding a consistent and robust finding across forensic and non-forensic populations. The psychopathic measures from either clinical diagnostic tools or self-report personality scales are positively related to proactive aggression (Cima & Raine, 2009; Reidy, Zeichner, Miller, & Martinez, 2007; Walsh, Swogger, & Kosson, 2009). However, less consistent findings are available on the association between psychopathic traits and reactive aggression across different population samples. Non-forensic individuals with higher psychopathic scores showed significantly greater reactive aggression in a laboratory aggression paradigm (Lotze, Veit, Anders, & Birbaumer, 2007; Reidy et al., 2007; Reidy, Zeichner, & Martinez, 2008; Reidy, Zeichner, & Seibert, 2011). On the contrary, forensic individuals with higher psychopathic scores showed significantly less reactive aggression using the same experimental paradigm (Veit et al., 2010). Other research on both youth and adult offenders demonstrated that psychopathy is more associated with proactive aggression than reactive aggression (Bezdjian, Tuvblad, Raine, & Baker, 2011; Cima & Raine, 2009; Lehmann & Ittel, 2012). However, a recent meta-analysis study revealed that an increased level of an individual’s psychopathic traits is associated with an enhanced propensity of both proactive aggression and reactive aggression—although the effect size is significantly larger for self-report scales in general population than that for clinical assessments in forensic population (Blais, Solodukhin, & Forth, 2014). Therefore, whether higher psychopathic traits facilitate reactive aggression remains inconclusive. Further research is needed to clarify the relationship between psychopathic traits and the risk of reactive aggression.

To date, reactive aggression has been widely studied in controlled laboratory settings using reliable and well-validated behavioral approaches (Krämer, Jansma, Tempelmann, & Munte, 2007; Krämer, Riba, Richter, & Münte, 2011; Lotze et al., 2007). The Taylor Aggression Paradigm (TAP) is one of the most frequently used measures of reactive aggression (Taylor, 1967). One recognized version of the TAP involves participants competing against a fictitious opponent in a reaction-time game in which they are asked to respond as fast as possible to win—whereby the outcome of the game is predetermined by the experimenter (Krämer, Büttner, Roth, & Münte, 2008). For each single trial of the game, one player has to select the intensity of a prospective punishment for the other player. A high-intensity punishment selected by the fictitious opponent is regarded as a provocation; it can elicit reactive aggression in participants—administering a similar high-level punishment against the opponent. Different types of aversive stimuli are used as effective punishment, including electric shock, unpleasant noise, and pneumatic pressure (Krämer et al., 2007, 2008; Lotze et al., 2007; Taylor, 1967; Veit et al., 2010).

A recent study further split the TAP into a passive and an active phase. For the passive phase, participants can only receive punishment when losing but without an opportunity to punish the opponent when winning, whereas in the active phase participants can punish the opponent when winning but without receiving any punishment even when losing (Beyens, Yu, Han, Zhang, & Zhou, 2015). This modification enhances the effectiveness of provocation and highly motivates subsequent reactive aggression since participants continuously experience high-level punishment without being able for immediate revenge.

A plethora of TAP studies have consistently demonstrated that people adopt a tit-for-tat strategy and exhibit significantly more reactive aggression after being provoked (Beyer, Munte, Gottlich, & Kramer, 2015; Brunnlieb, Munte, Kramer, Tempelmann, & Heldmann, 2013; Krämer et al., 2007, 2011). Previous studies allowed participants to retaliate against provokers, which mirrored most revenge situations occurring in real daily lives (Beyens et al., 2015; Chester & DeWall, 2016; Ping et al., 2018). However, as known from some other types of violence—including school shootings—some offenders, when cornered, might take revenge on innocent people to vent their frustration caused by someone else. Although a few studies have investigated such a phenomenon in a controlled laboratory setting (Reijntjes, Kamphuis, Thomaes, Bushman, & Telch, 2013; Reijntjes, Thomaes et al., 2013), it remains unknown what kind of individuals are more inclined to retaliate against innocent people after provocation. Considering that psychopathic traits are closely associated with aggressive behavior (Lotze et al., 2007; Reidy et al., 2007, 2008; Reidy et al., 2011), individual differences of psychopathic traits might have an impact on transferring reactive aggression to innocent people. However, as far as we know, few studies have investigated this phenomenon. Hence, a need exists in identifying the behavioral and especially the neuropsychological signatures for antisocial-aggressive behavior toward the innocents by people with different levels of psychopathic traits.

Measuring different event-related potentials (ERPs) components while performing the TAP allows investigating the neuropsychological underpinnings of reactive aggression. First, the N2 component—a negative deflection distributed around 200∼300 ms post-onset—has been frequently associated with reactive aggression as measured with the TAP (Beyens et al., 2015; Krämer et al., 2008; Nieuwenhuis, Slagter, von Geusau, Heslenfeld, & Holroyd, 2005). For example, N2 amplitudes induced by a high relative to a low provocation are only present in individuals with higher aggressive traits (Krämer et al., 2008).

Second, the feedback-related negativity (FRN) component, occurring at about 250 ms post-stimulus, is usually observed during the TAP outcome stage (Beyens et al., 2015; Krämer et al., 2008; Ping et al., 2018; Wiswede et al., 2011). The FRN is being known as an index of a feedback valence evaluation (Müller, Möller, Rodriguez-Fornells, & Münte, 2005; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005). A more negative FRN is expected when people experience or observe a negative outcome (e.g., monetary loss, game loss) (Beyens et al., 2015; Fukushima & Hiraki, 2006; Heldmann, Russeler, & Munte, 2008; Ping et al., 2018; Yeung, Holroyd, & Cohen, 2005). Further, a more negative FRN elicited by losing a trial is only found in individuals with high compared to low aggressive behavior (Krämer et al., 2008).

Third, the P3 component is a positive deflection observed at about 300∼600 ms post-stimulus onset (Kim & Jung, 2014) and is associated with attention-orienting processes, memory operations, and inhibitory control (Friedman, Cycowicz, & Gaeta, 2001; Polich, 2007). A decreased P3 component is closely associated with antisocial and aggressive behavior (Bingren, Chanchan, Qisha, Guorong, & Wei, 2016; Gao & Raine, 2009; Pasion, Fernandes, Pereira, & Barbosa, 2018; Verona & Bresin, 2015). Moreover, an atypical P3 component is also observed for individuals with higher psychopathic traits in forensic and non-forensic samples (Anderson & Stanford, 2012; Carlson, Thai, & McLarnon, 2009; Drislane, Vaidyanathan, & Patrick, 2013; Kim & Jung, 2014).

In this study, we combined a modified TAP with ERP recordings to examine how individual differences of psychopathic traits are related to the behavior of transferring reactive aggression to innocent people after strong provocation. We also asked the same participants to complete a subsequent behavioral experiment with a low provocation to examine whether individuals with higher psychopathic traits would show more aggression toward unrelated people regardless of the level of provocation. Participants from an undergraduate student population were divided into a high psychopathic (HP) traits group and a low psychopathic (LP) traits group based on the Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (LSRP) (Levenson, Kiehl, & Fitzpatrick, 1995). They were asked to compete with two fictitious opponents in a reaction-time game during two phases. For the passive phase, participants were provoked by the first opponent who selected high-intensity punishment most of the time and were punished after losing a trial—but were unable to punish the opponent even after winning a trial. During the active phase, they played against the second opponent, who was not responsible for the provocation in the passive phase.

At the behavioral level, we hypothesized that the HP group (compared to the LP group) selects more often a high-intensive punishment for innocent opponents in the active phase only after being strongly provoked in the passive phase. At the neural level, we predicted that the HP group (compared to the LP group) displays a larger N2 amplitude and a smaller P3 amplitude—reflecting the level of aggressiveness and impulsive-antisocial behavior—while deciding the punishment intensity for the innocent opponent. Further, we expected a more negative FRN for losing than for winning trials in the HP group (compared to the LP group) after receiving feedback about the outcome of the trial—reflecting the negative valence of being not able to punish after losing the trial.

Section snippets

Participants

The current study used the LSRP to measure individual psychopathic traits in a college student sample. Questionnaire data were obtained from 272 undergraduate students (122 females, 150 males, age [mean ± standard deviation]: 19.53 ± 1.43 years). Participants were sorted based on their total LSRP scores (Shao & Lee, 2017): participants scoring at the top 25% (≥ 59) or bottom 25% (≤ 48) within the total sample were invited to participate in the ERP experiment. Many of the participants who met

Questionnaire results

The questionnaire results showed that the HP compared to the LP group demonstrated significantly higher LSRP scores on primary (t (48) = 12.37, p < 0.001, d = 3.50), secondary (t (48) = 9.03, p < 0.001, d = 2.56), and total (t (48) = 16.68, p < 0.001, d = 4.72) psychopathy (Table 1). Further, the HP group showed remarkably higher RPQ scores than the LP group on proactive (t (48) = 5.03, p < 0.001, d = 1.42), reactive (t (48) = 5.17, p < 0.001, d = 1.46), and total (t (48) = 6.22, p < 0.001, d =

Discussion

In this study, we combined a modified TAP with ERP recordings to investigate how individual psychopathic traits would impact on the tendency of transferring reactive aggression to an innocent person after being provoked by another person. Our behavioral results showed that individuals with higher psychopathic traits were more likely to make high-intensity punishment decisions toward an innocent person after high-level provocation compared to low-level provocation. Our ERP results demonstrated

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31500907).

Author contributions statements

QY and YW developed this study. YW programmed the experiment. YW, BZ, SY collected the data. YW performed the data analyses. QY and YW drafted the initial manuscript. FK, QY, and TX made critical revisions and polished the article. All the authors have read the final version of the manuscript and approved of its publication.

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors declared that they do not have any conflicts of interest in this paper.

References (94)

  • Y.Y. Kim et al.

    Reduced frontal activity during response inhibition in individuals with psychopathic traits: An sLORETA study

    Biological Psychology

    (2014)
  • U.M. Krämer et al.

    Tit-for-tat: The neural basis of reactive aggression

    Neuroimage

    (2007)
  • A. Lehmann et al.

    Aggressive behavior and measurement of psychopathy in female inmates of German prisons--a preliminary study

    International Journal of Law and Psychiatry

    (2012)
  • M. Lotze et al.

    Evidence for a different role of the ventral and dorsal medial prefrontal cortex for social reactive aggression: An interactive fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2007)
  • S.V. Müller et al.

    Brain potentials related to self-generated and external information used for performance monitoring

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2005)
  • R. Pasion et al.

    Antisocial behaviour and psychopathy: Uncovering the externalizing link in the P3 modulation

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2018)
  • J. Polich

    Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2007)
  • D.E. Reidy et al.

    Psychopathy and aggression: Examining the role of psychopathy factors in predicting laboratory aggression under hostile and instrumental conditions

    Journal of Research in Personality

    (2007)
  • A. Reijntjes et al.

    Youths’ displaced aggression against in- and out-group peers: An experimental examination

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2013)
  • R. Veit et al.

    Aberrant social and cerebral responding in a competitive reaction time paradigm in criminal psychopaths

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • E. Verona et al.

    Aggression proneness: Transdiagnostic processes involving negative valence and cognitive systems

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2015)
  • A. Vila-Ballo et al.

    Neurophysiological correlates of error monitoring and inhibitory processing in juvenile violent offenders

    Biological Psychology

    (2014)
  • N.E. Anderson et al.

    Demonstrating emotional processing differences in psychopathy using affective ERP modulation

    Psychophysiology

    (2012)
  • J.J. Asscher et al.

    The relationship between juvenile psychopathic traits, delinquency and (violent) recidivism: A meta-analysis

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines

    (2011)
  • S.D. Benning et al.

    Factor structure of the psychopathic personality inventory: Validity and implications for clinical assessment

    Psychological Assessment

    (2003)
  • L. Berkowitz

    The experience of anger as a parallel process in the display of impulsive,“angry” aggression

    Aggression: Theoretical and Empirical Reviews

    (1983)
  • E. Bertoletti et al.

    A general population twin study of conduct problems and the auditory P300 waveform

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2014)
  • B.A. Bettencourt et al.

    Gender differences in aggression as a function of provocation: A meta-analysis

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1996)
  • U. Beyens et al.

    The strength of a remorseful heart: Psychological and neural basis of how apology emolliates reactive aggression and promotes forgiveness

    Frontiers in Psychology

    (2015)
  • F. Beyer et al.

    Orbitofrontal cortex reactivity to angry facial expression in a social interaction correlates with aggressive behavior

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2015)
  • S. Bezdjian et al.

    The genetic and environmental covariation among psychopathic personality traits, and reactive and proactive aggression in childhood

    Child Development

    (2011)
  • Z. Bingren et al.

    Processing of facial expressions of emotions in Antisocial, Narcissistic, and Schizotypal personality disorders: An event-related potential study

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2016)
  • A.J. Bishara et al.

    Reducing bias and error in the correlation coefficient due to nonnormality

    Educational and Psychological Measurement

    (2015)
  • J. Blais et al.

    A meta-analysis exploring the relationship between psychopathy and instrumental versus reactive violence

    Criminal Justice and Behavior

    (2014)
  • I.A. Brazil et al.

    Differentiating psychopathy from general antisociality using the P3 as a psychophysiological correlate of attentional allocation

    PloS One

    (2012)
  • C. Brunnlieb et al.

    Vasopressin modulates neural responses during human reactive aggression

    Social Neuroscience

    (2013)
  • S.R. Carlson et al.

    Visual P3 amplitude and self-reported psychopathic personality traits: Frontal reduction is associated with self-centered impulsivity

    Psychophysiology

    (2009)
  • K.A. Chase et al.

    Categorizing partner-violent men within the reactive-proactive typology model

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2001)
  • A. Chen et al.

    The timing of cognitive control in partially incongruent categorization

    Human Brain Mapping

    (2008)
  • D.S. Chester et al.

    The pleasure of revenge: Retaliatory aggression arises from a neural imbalance toward reward

    Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

    (2016)
  • M. Cima et al.

    Psychopaths know right from wrong but don’t care

    Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

    (2010)
  • H.M. Cleckley

    The mask of sanity

    Postgraduate Medicine

    (1951)
  • M.H. Davis

    A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy

    (1980)
  • A. Debowska et al.

    Gender differences in the correlates of reactive aggression

    Polish Psychological Bulletin

    (2015)
  • K.A. Dodge et al.

    Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1987)
  • L.E. Drislane et al.

    Reduced cortical call to arms differentiates psychopathy from antisocial personality disorder

    Psychological Medicine

    (2013)
  • J.F. Edens et al.

    Psychopathic, not psychopath: Taxometric evidence for the dimensional structure of psychopathy

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2006)
  • Cited by (3)

    View full text