Elsevier

Biological Psychology

Volume 103, December 2014, Pages 283-291
Biological Psychology

Mind your thoughts: Associations between self-generated thoughts and stress-induced and baseline levels of cortisol and alpha-amylase

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.10.004Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We examine the association of thought processes and the physiological response to psychosocial stress.

  • More negatively toned emotional thoughts and more social temporal thoughts with a past focus were associated with increased cortisol and alpha-amylase levels, both after stress and at baseline.

  • More social temporal thoughts with a future focus had an overall attenuating effect on the levels of both stress markers.

  • Our results indicate a fundamental link between the thoughts and stress levels we experience.

Abstract

Stress is a major health burden in today's society. Research shows that negative cognitive styles are associated with increased stress reactivity, low mood and accelerated cellular aging. Our study sought to unravel the relationship between the content of self-generated thoughts and psychosocial stress measured in terms of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and sympathetic activity. Features of self-generated thoughts were assessed using thought sampling while participants performed cognitive tasks following a stress induction or in a baseline condition. More negatively toned emotional thoughts and more social temporal thoughts with a past focus were associated with increased cortisol and alpha-amylase levels, both after stress and at baseline. More social temporal thoughts with a future focus, on the other hand, had an overall attenuating effect on the levels of both stress markers. Our results indicate a fundamental link between the thoughts and stress levels we experience. Understanding the mechanisms governing this mind-body association may have important implications for understanding and counteracting the high incidence of stress-related disorders in today's society.

Introduction

Stress is a major health factor in today's complex and fast-paced society. Stress disorders are among the most commonly occurring of all mental disorders (WHO International Consortium in Psychiatric Epidemiology, 2000) and the estimated costs for stress at the workplace have been calculated to exceed US$ 300 billion annually in the US economy (American Institute of Stress, 2001). However, not everybody develops stress disorders, raising the question as to the resilience factors protecting these individuals. We suggest that one key to successful coping with stressful life events lies in the thoughts that individuals generate consequent to these events. Accordingly, the goal of this study was to unravel the associations between thoughts and stress, aiming specifically to identify those types of post-stress thoughts that protect individuals from the negative impact of a stressor.

We focused our investigation on cognitive and affective experiences produced by the individual without a referent in the immediate sensory environment known as self-generated thoughts (SGT) (Smallwood, 2013). These unconstrained thoughts cover a wide category of experiences including daydreaming, mind wandering, rumination and planning, and have recently become an important focus of research at the level of both psychology and neuroscience. Capitalizing on smart-phone technologies, experience-sampling studies have found that SGT such as mind wandering or daydreaming are common in the laboratory and in daily life (Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010). SGT have also been implicated in states of creativity (Baird et al., 2012) and unhappiness (Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010) demonstrating that the capacity for SGT plays an important role in human life.

A similar property of complex self-generated behavior is evident in neural processing. Almost all of the neural systems that exhibit co-ordinated responses as part of an external task show the same patterns of spatially organized behavior during periods of wakeful rest (Smith et al., 2009). Moreover, the behavior of these resting state networks is predictive of important aspects of an individual's psychological profile including their meta-cognitive accuracy (Baird, Smallwood, Gorgolewski, & Margulies, 2013), their capacity for reading (Smallwood, Gorgolewski, et al., 2013) and various psychological disorders. Recent research, therefore, has highlighted that SGT is a natural and influential property at both psychological and biological levels, making it important to understand its relationship to stress.

SGT have a documented relationship to negative affect. Laboratory studies have shown that the induction of negative mood increases participants’ SGT levels (Smallwood et al., 2009a, Smallwood and O’Connor, 2011) and that this effect is stronger in individuals who are chronically unhappy (Smallwood & O’Connor, 2011). Experience sampling studies have found the occurrence of SGT to be associated with more concurrent negative mood, which can persist for several hours (Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010). The emotional aspects of SGT also have implications at the biological level. Women who reported the highest frequency of negatively toned mind wandering had significantly shorter telomerase length, an epigenetic marker of cellular aging (Epel et al., 2013). An unhappy mood not only increases SGT, it also shifts the temporal focus of the experience toward the past (Smallwood & O’Connor, 2011). In a recent examination, we found that while a focus on the past led to a reduction in positive mood, a focus on the future triggered more positive mood several minutes later (Ruby, Smallwood, Engen, & Singer, 2013). This has led to the hypothesis that the content of SGT can determine its relationship to functional outcomes (Andrews-Hanna, Smallwood, & Spreng, 2014).

Considering the potential of our thoughts to impact on our wellbeing – in both a beneficial and a detrimental fashion – we aimed to examine the relationship between the content of SGT and an acute physiological stress experience as well as the process of recovery from it. Beyond sympathetic nervous system (SNS) activation as unspecific sign of general arousal, stress experience requires the activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal- (HPA) axis resulting in cortisol release (Chrousos, 2009, Hellhammer et al., 2009).

Previous studies examining associations between cognitive processes and cortisol regulation in community samples have focused mainly on rumination and yielded inconsistent results. Rumination has been defined as compulsively focused attention on the symptoms of one's distress and its possible causes and consequences as opposed to its solutions (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008). According to the Perseverative Cognition Hypothesis by Brosschot, Gerin, and Thayer (2006), rumination entails an ongoing mental representation of a stressor, which should be associated with prolonged activation of stress-related physiological systems. Indeed, state rumination measured after laboratory-induced social evaluative threat was linked to higher reactive cortisol levels (Zoccola, Dickerson, & Zaldivar, 2008). Likewise, the induction of rumination led to reduced diurnal cortisol decline in individuals scoring higher on depressive mood (Kuehner, Huffziger, & Liebsch, 2009). When observing rumination as a trait, individuals with high levels of habitual rumination in response to negative mood showed an attenuated cortisol awakening response (Kuehner, Holzhauer, & Huffziger, 2007) and reduced cortisol reactivity to social evaluative threat in the lab (Zoccola et al., 2008), while stress-related trait rumination was positively correlated with evening cortisol levels (Rydstedt, Cropley, Devereux, & Michalianou, 2009).

To experimentally induce stress in the current study, participants underwent the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) (Kirschbaum et al., 1993, Kudielka et al., 2007), a psychosocial laboratory stressor. Subsequent to stress induction, SGT were assessed using a thought sampling technique that addressed the temporal (past-future), emotional (negative-positive) and social (referent of thought self-other) features of thoughts. To identify statistically independent patterns of SGT, these reports were decomposed using Principal Component Analysis. Our previous research using the same technique revealed three distinct covariance patterns (Ruby, Smallwood, Engen, et al., 2013): two reflected social temporal thoughts with either a past or a future focus. A third covariance pattern reflected an emotional element to cognition, i.e., whether thoughts were positively or negatively toned. In order to provide a stress-independent thought sampling baseline, participants came to the laboratory on a separate day and SGT were assessed in the same way but following a period of rest. Repeated measurements of salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase as indicators of HPA-axis and SNS activation were captured throughout both testing sessions.

Based on previous research exploring SGT and rumination, we hypothesized social temporal thoughts with a past focus and negative in tone, a pattern of cognition that may be considered an index of rumination, to be positively associated with the total impact and duration of stress-induced cortisol and alpha-amylase levels. In contrast, because future thinking is neither a cause nor a consequence of unhappy mood and may in fact reduce negative affect (Ruby et al., 2013a, Smallwood and O’Connor, 2011), we expected social temporal thoughts with a future focus to be linked to a relatively decreased physiological response to and accelerated physiological recovery from the stressor.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants between 18 and 35 years of age were recruited by posting ads on an electronic billboard of the city of Leipzig. 99 participants underwent the baseline phase of this study and provided thought sampling data (52 women; mean age ± SD: 25.30 ± 3.85 years). Out of these 99 participants, 46 (selected on the basis of availability) additionally took part in the stress session (23 women; mean age ± SD: 25.70 ± 3.61 years). Given a potential effect on cortisol activity, regular recreational drug

Results

In an initial reliability check, we verified the efficacy of our stress paradigm to elicit significant cortisol stress responses. A subsistent physiological stress response has been defined by an average cortisol peak of at least 1.5 nmol/l over the baseline (Miller, Plessow, Kirschbaum, & Stalder, 2013). With an average increase of 7.5 nmol/l and minimum increases of 1.5 nmol/l in 41 out of 46 participants (89%) the efficacy of our manipulation is adequately confirmed (Fig. 1B). We also showed

Discussion

Our study sought to unravel the relationship between self-generated thoughts (SGT) and stress, aiming specifically to identify those types of post-stress thoughts that may protect individuals from the negative impact of a stressful event. We examined the association of SGT with stress-induced and baseline levels of cortisol and alpha-amylase, and found levels of both stress markers to vary depending on the content of SGT. In detail, more social temporal thoughts with a past focus were

References (60)

  • K.M. Ross et al.

    How stable are diurnal cortisol activity indices in healthy individuals? Evidence from three multi-wave studies

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2014)
  • J. Smallwood et al.

    Segmenting the stream of consciousness: The psychological correlates of temporal structures in the time series data of a continuous performance task

    Brain and Cognition

    (2008)
  • J. Smallwood et al.

    When is your head at? An exploration of the factors associated with the temporal focus of the wandering mind

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2009)
  • J. Smallwood et al.

    Letting go of the present: Mind-wandering is associated with reduced delay discounting

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2013)
  • J. Smallwood et al.

    Self-reflection and the temporal focus of the wandering mind

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2011)
  • J. Smallwood et al.

    Escaping the here and now: Evidence for a role of the default mode network in perceptually decoupled thought

    NeuroImage

    (2013)
  • E.J. Wamsley et al.

    Dreaming and offline memory processing

    Current Biology: CB

    (2010)
  • American Institute of Stress

    The quandary of job stress compensation

    Health and Stress Newsletters

    (2001)
  • J.R. Andrews-Hanna

    The brain's default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation

    Neuroscientist

    (2012)
  • J.R. Andrews-Hanna et al.

    The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance

    Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

    (2014)
  • B. Baird et al.

    Medial and lateral networks in anterior prefrontal cortex support metacognitive ability for memory and perception

    Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

    (2013)
  • B. Baird et al.

    Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation

    Psychological Science

    (2012)
  • R.B. Cattell et al.

    P-technique demonstrated in determining psycho-physiological source traits in a normal individual

    Psychometrika

    (1947)
  • G.P. Chrousos

    Stress and disorders of the stress system

    Nature Reviews Endocrinology

    (2009)
  • M.F. Dallman et al.

    Hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis

  • E.R. de Kloet et al.

    Stress and the brain: From adaptation to disease. Nature reviews

    Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • S.S. Dickerson et al.

    Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2004)
  • B. Egloff et al.

    Relationships between time of day, day of the week, and positive mood: Exploring the role of the mood measure

    Motivation and Emotion

    (1995)
  • E.S. Epel et al.

    Wandering minds and aging cells

    Clinical Psychological Science

    (2013)
  • B.H. Friedman et al.

    Idiodynamic profiles of cardiovascular activity: A P-technique approach

    Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science: The Official Journal of the Pavlovian Society

    (2003)
  • Cited by (60)

    • Mind wandering in anxiety disorders: A status report

      2023, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    • Trait and state self-compassion interactively predict cortisol recovery following an acute stressor in healthy males

      2022, Psychoneuroendocrinology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Level 2 model:π0j = β00 + β01 × Conditionj + r0j,π1j = β10 + β11 × Conditionj + r1j,π2j = β20 + β21 × Conditionj + r2j,π3j = β30 + β31 × Conditionj + r3j,π4j = β40 + β41 × Conditionj + r4j· For cortisol levels, a similar analytic approach was applied; linear, quadratic, cubic, and piece-wise models (modeling activation to peak and recovery from peak, e.g., Engert et al., 2014) were evaluated for cortisol trajectories from − 10 to + 50 min. This resulted in the acceptance of the cubic model (AIC = 145.94), which showed a significantly better fit than the other models tested (all ps < 0.001).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Joint first authors who contributed equally to the work.

    View full text