Elsevier

Appetite

Volume 138, 1 July 2019, Pages 198-203
Appetite

Anticipation of aerobic exercise increases planned energy intake for a post-exercise meal

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2019.03.035Get rights and content

Abstract

In many situations, meals are planned (i.e. what and how much) before they are eaten, but how exercise influences this planning is unknown. Therefore, this study investigated whether anticipation of an exercise session alters food intake planned for post-exercise. Forty (16 male) regular exercisers (mean ± SD; age 23.3 ± 5.6 y, BMI 22.7 ± 3.3 kg/m2, body fat 25.6 ± 7.6%) completed the study. Subjects arrived ≥3 h post-prandial and were given two hypothetical scenarios for the following day: 1) morning rest (REST), or 2) morning rest with the addition of 1 h of hard aerobic exercise at 10:00–11:00 (EXERCISE). For each scenario subjects had to plan their lunch, to consume at 12:00, by serving themselves cheesy tomato pasta and chocolate buttons. Scenarios were randomised and separated by 5 min and foods were not consumed. EXERCISE increased total energy served by 24% (EXERCISE 3308 ± 1217 kJ; REST 2663 ± 924 kJ; P < 0.001), with increases in energy served from both pasta (+25%; P < 0.001) and chocolate buttons (+20%; P = 0.024). These results suggest aerobic exercise increases planned post-exercise energy intake, if a meal is planned in advance of exercise. Future research should examine the impact of exercise on meal planning at other meals, as well as how this behaviour impacts weight loss with exercise training.

Introduction

The most recent public health statistics suggest that the prevalence of overweight and obesity continue to rise, with 61% of UK adults currently classified as overweight or obese (Health Survey for England, 2016). Weight gain occurs due to chronic positive energy balance (i.e. energy intake greater than energy expenditure), leading to accumulation of fat in adipose tissue (Schrauwen, 2007). Increasing physical activity, particularly aerobic activity, is one method of increasing energy expenditure that has been suggested to assist with weight management (Caudwell et al., 2011). The premise of this strategy is that the accumulation of energy expended through physical activity manifests in a negative energy balance and subsequent reduction in body fat levels (Caudwell et al., 2011).

Clearly, the success of a weight loss strategy involving increased exercise will depend on the degree of compensation through the other components of energy balance (i.e. energy intake, resting energy expenditure and of physical activity; Caudwell et al., 2011). Acute exercise studies have typically reported a transient reduction in subjective appetite (Broom, Stensel, Bishop, Burns, & Miyashita, 2007; Pomerleau, Imbeault, Parker, & Doucet, 2004) and ideal portion size (Farah et al. 2012) during/after exercise, with minimal effect on subsequent energy intake compared to a resting control trial (Schubert, Desbrow, Sabapathy, & Leveritt, 2013). Whilst some studies report a small increase in absolute energy intake (i.e. total energy consumed) after exercise (Martins et al., 2007a, 2007b; Pomerleau et al., 2004; Shorten, Wallman, & Guelfi, 2009), relative energy intake (energy consumed minus energy expended through exercise/rest) is consistently reduced by exercise. Therefore, acute exercise studies suggest exercise produces an environment conducive to weight loss by increasing energy expenditure without a compensatory increase in energy intake.

However, chronic exercise interventions (i.e. ≥8 weeks) have typically not observed the anticipated weight loss that would be expected given the acute effects of exercise on relative energy intake (King, Hopkins, Caudwell, Stubbs, & Blundell, 2008; Turner et al. 2010; Wu, Gao, Chen, & van Dam, 2009). Typically, there is an initial weight loss, however, after this, the rate of weight loss attenuates or weight becomes stable over time (Curioni & Lourenco, 2005; Wu et al., 2009). Whilst there is likely a reduced energy requirement due to the reduction in body mass over time, these studies also suggest there is some alteration in the other components of energy balance to compensate in some way for the energy expended through exercise training. Given that non-prescribed physical activity energy expenditure (Turner et al., 2010) and resting metabolic rate (Lee et al. 2009; Speakman & Selman, 2003) do not appear to change with exercise training, alterations in dietary intake/eating behaviour have been suggested as the likely cause of this effect (Turner et al. 2010).

Aside from what we eat, a critical factor is how much we eat. Factors influencing portion size selection strongly affect energy intake, and therefore represent a crucial aspect of energy balance (Brunstrom, 2011). Previous studies that have investigated the relationship between exercise and energy intake have employed an ad-libitum approach to assess energy intake. In this approach, subjects are presented with a variety of food items in excess amounts and are asked to eat or drink until satiated. In day-to-day living, this type of eating occasion is relatively rare for most humans, with meals generally involving some planning of the type and/or the amount of food selected, in advance of the eating occasion (Brunstrom, 2011). Additionally, food choice is generally reduced in a laboratory environment.

Interestingly, Werle, Wansink, and Payne (2011) reported that participants who answered a series of questions related to exercise served themselves more snacks, and therefore more calories, than those in a control group whose questions were unrelated to exercise. In most cases, exercise sessions are scheduled in advance of being undertaken (i.e. individuals know that they will exercise and likely think about the exercise), meaning the size/nature of any meals prepared/cooked in advance of exercise might be influenced by the knowledge of the upcoming exercise session. However, the design of most previous exercise studies does not allow any planning behaviour in the context of exercise to be directly evaluated in advance of the session. More recently, Sim et al. (2014) reported that inactive overweight males scoring high for dietary restraint increased energy intake at a snack before exercise. These results suggest that exercise might increase energy intake (or planned energy intake) when decisions are made in advance of exercise, although the training and/or weight status of the volunteers may have influenced the results.

Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine how exercise influences meals planned for the post-exercise period of regularly exercisers by providing subjects with hypothetical exercise and rest scenarios and asking them to plan their post-exercise meal. It was hypothesised that subjects would plan to consume more energy after exercise than after rest.

Section snippets

Subjects

Twenty-four females (age 21 ± 3 years; BMI 22.0 ± 2.8 kg/m2; body fat % 30.1 ± 4.5) and sixteen males (age 26 ± 8 years; BMI 24.0 ± 3.7 kg/m2; body fat % 18.3 ± 5.5) completed this study, which was approved by the Loughborough University Ethics Approvals (Human Participants) Sub Committee (reference number: SSEHS-1917). Before participation, subjects provided written consent, and completed a health screen questionnaire and the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ; Stunkard & Messick, 1985)

Subjective appetite

Pre-trial values for hunger (Z = -1.013; P = 0.311), fullness (Z = -0.014; P = 0.989), DTE (Z = -0.587; P = 0.557) and PFC (Z = -1.356; P = 0.175) were not different between REST and EXERCISE trials (Table 2).

Portion size selection

There was no trial order effect for the total energy content served, with similar amounts served for trial 1 and trial 2 (Trial 1 2990 ± 1096 kJ; Trial 2 2982 ± 1160 kJ; Z = -0.168; P = 0.867). Similarly, there was no difference between trial 1 and trial 2 for energy served from pasta (Z

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of exercise on post-exercise meal planning. The main finding was that individuals chose a larger portion size (∼24% increase in energy content of food served) to consume after a hypothetical future aerobic exercise scenario compared to a hypothetical rest scenario.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the effect of aerobic exercise on meal planning behaviour in anticipation of exercise. Previous investigations have

Acknowledgements

L.J. James is part of the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Leicester Biomedical Research Center, which is a partnership between University Hospitals of Leicester NHS Trust, Loughborough University, and the University of Leicester. This report is independent research by the National Institute for Health Research. The views expressed in this publication are those ofthe authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the National Institute for Health Research, or the Department of

References (40)

Cited by (6)

  • Accentuated early postprandial satiety in people with spinal cord injury versus able-bodied controls

    2021, Appetite
    Citation Excerpt :

    The HE (2513 kJ, 10 g carbohydrate, 22 g fat, 0.8 g protein) and LE (1008 kJ, 100 g carbohydrate, 22 g fat, 0.8 g protein) preload drinks (Long et al., 2002) were prepared on the morning of the trial. The homogenous pasta meal was prepared the day before the trial, using consistent cooking procedures (Barutcu et al., 2019), before being refrigerated. To determine ad libitum energy intake, participants were presented with the pasta in a large Pyrex bowl, which was served chilled and in excess of predicted consumption (4339 kJ, 59% carbohydrate, 20% fat, 16% protein, 5% fibre).

  • Exercise-related factors that influence post-exercise energy intake: A psychological perspective

    2020, Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport
    Citation Excerpt :

    A possible explanation for this finding provided by the authors was that participants provided with the high energy expenditure information may have felt more deserving of a reward, and consequently experienced a greater license to consume, whilst the similar ratings of hunger may suggest that participants were not expecting, or planning, to eat more.45 Barutcu et al.46 have corroborated these findings, such that men and women served themselves more food from a pasta and chocolate test meal following a hypothetical ‘hard aerobic’ session than following a period of rest. In other work, Werle et al.47 found that labelling exercise as a scenic 1.6 km walk (which was perceived as ‘fun’), rather than an ‘exercise’ walk, led to reduced subsequent intake of unhealthy snacks, in middle-aged women.

  • Cold-water effects on energy balance in healthy women during aqua-cycling

    2021, International Journal of Sport Nutrition and Exercise Metabolism
  • Planned Aerobic Exercise Increases Energy Intake at the Preceding Meal

    2020, Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise
View full text