What can hunger teach us about drug craving? A comparative analysis of the two constructs

https://doi.org/10.1016/0146-6402(92)90006-AGet rights and content

Abstract

While few deny the prevalence of craving among drug-dependent individuals, many theorists have suggested that craving has outlived its usefulness as a meaningful psychological construct. The prevailing notion is that craving reflects an underlying biologic need, but the empirical support for this appears weak. To examine this concern, we review a similar psychological construct: hunger. An intuitive view is that hunger reflects the need to eat due to energy depletion. In this paper, we outline conceptual expectations of the constructs of craving and hunger, and review evidence suggesting that neither craving nor hunger (1) is a simple reflection of underlying biologic need, (2) has reliable physiological correlates or controls, (3) is strongly linked to affective state, or (4) reliably predicts consumption (food or drug). Rather than abandon the study of a compelling psychological construct, researchers ought to re-examine their expectations of craving, and re-focus their attempts to understand the role of craving in drug use. A review of the hunger literature also suggests several fruitful lines for investigation of craving.

References (128)

  • J.M. De Castro et al.

    Subjective hunger relationships with meal patterns in the spontaneous feeding behavior of humans: evidence for a causal connection

    Physiology and Behavior

    (1988)
  • J.M. De Castro et al.

    A microregulatory analysis of spontaneous human feeding patterns

    Physiology and Behavior

    (1985)
  • J.M. De Castro et al.

    Spontaneous human meal patterns are related to preprandial factors regardless of natural environmental constraints

    Physiology and Behavior

    (1986)
  • J.J. Franchina et al.

    Effects of deprivation on salivary flow in the apparent absence of food stimuli

    Appetite

    (1988)
  • R. Hodgson et al.

    Alcohol dependence and the priming effect

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1979)
  • M.E. Jarvik et al.

    Potentiation of cigarette craving and satisfaction by two types of meals

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1989)
  • L.T. Kozlowski et al.

    “Cravings” are ambiguous: ask about urges or desires

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1989)
  • R. Larson et al.

    Bulimia: disturbed patterns of solitude

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1985)
  • M.R. Lowe et al.

    Restraint, disinhibition, hunger and negative affect eating

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1988)
  • A.M. Ludwig

    Pavlov's “bells” and alcohol craving

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1986)
  • R. Mattes

    Hunger ratings are not a valid proxy measure of reported food intake in humans

    Appetite

    (1990)
  • R. Niaura et al.

    Responses to smoking-related stimuli and early relapse to smoking

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1989)
  • H.J. Rankin et al.

    The concept of craving and its measurement

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1979)
  • P.J. Rogers et al.

    Breakdown of dietary restraint following mere exposure to food stimuli: interrelationships between restraint, hunger, salivation, and food intake

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1989)
  • B.J. Rolls et al.

    The specificity of satiety: the influence of foods of different macronutrient content on the development of satiety

    Physiology and Behavior

    (1988)
  • B.J. Rolls et al.

    Sensory specific satiety in man

    Physiology and Behavior

    (1981)
  • B.J. Rolls et al.

    Pleasantness changes and food intake in a varied four course meal

    Appetite

    (1984)
  • J.E. Rose et al.

    Sensory blockade of smoking satisfaction

    Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior

    (1985)
  • J.L. Saumet et al.

    Heat loss and anticipatory finger vasoconstriction induced by smoking of a single cigarette

    Physiology and Behavior

    (1985)
  • J.E. Sherman et al.

    Urges/craving to smoke: preliminary results from withdrawing and continuing smokers

    Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1986)
  • S. Shiffman

    A cluster-analytic classification of smoking relapse episodes

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1986)
  • H.J. Alban Davies et al.

    Metabolic response to low- and very-low calorie diets

    American Journal of Clinical Nutrition

    (1989)
  • T.B. Baker et al.

    The motivation to use drugs: a psychobiological analysis of urges

  • K.E. Bauman et al.

    The relationship between the consequences adolescents expect from smoking and their behavior: a factor analysis with panel data

    Journal of Applied Social Psychology

    (1984)
  • R.C. Bolles

    A functionalistic approach to feeding

  • D.A. Booth

    Hunger and satiety as conditioned reflexes

  • T.H. Brandon et al.

    The process of smoking relapse

  • S.A. Brown

    Expectancies versus background in the prediction of college drinking patterns

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1985)
  • H. Bruch

    Eating disorders

    (1973)
  • R. Bunker

    Stress and nicotine intake: underlying mechanisms and processes

    (1990)
  • W.B. Cannon et al.

    An explanation of hunger

    American Journal of Physiology

    (1912)
  • A.R. Childress et al.

    Classically conditioned responses in opioid and cocaine dependence: A role in relapse?

  • A.R. Childress et al.

    Mood states can elicit conditioned withdrawal and craving in opiate abuse patients

  • S. Cohen

    The chemical brain: The neurochemistry of addictive disorders

    (1988)
  • M.L. Cooper et al.

    Coping, expectancies, and alcohol abuse: a test of social learning formulations

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (1988)
  • R. Coronas et al.

    Modified total fasting and obesity: results of a multicentric study

    International Journal of Obesity

    (1982)
  • L.M. Eriksen et al.

    Conditioned abstinence in alcoholics: a controlled experiment

    International Journal of the Addictions

    (1984)
  • R.M. Gilbert et al.

    Early effects of quitting smoking

    Psychopharmacology

    (1982)
  • F.B. Glaser

    Psychologic vs. pharmacologic heroin dependence

    New England Journal of Medicine

    (1974)
  • M.S. Goldman et al.

    Expectancy theory: thinking about drinking

  • Cited by (53)

    • Craving and opioid use disorder: A scoping review

      2019, Drug and Alcohol Dependence
      Citation Excerpt :

      The report resulting from that meeting acknowledged the importance of craving as it related to alcohol use, but ultimately recommended that the term ‘craving’ be eliminated from the scientific literature due to confusion arising from its various ‘everyday connotations’ (Supplementary Materials, Appendix A). Since that time there has been continued debate regarding the clinical utility of the concept of craving as it relates to substance use disorders (Kassel and Shiffman, 1992; Kozlowski and Wilkinson, 1987; Pickens and Johanson, 1992; Sayette et al., 2000; Tiffany and Wray, 2009, 2012). However, despite these challenges, drug craving remains a salient and important patient-reported experience as reflected by a survey at a recent public meeting hosted by the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) in collaboration with the National Institute of Drug Abuse (NIDA) (see meeting slides located at Food and Drug Administration, 2018a).

    • Neuroscience of drug craving for addiction medicine: From circuits to therapies

      2016, Progress in Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Substance use disorder as a brain disease is characterized by persistent drug-seeking and drug-taking behaviors despite significant negative consequences in physical, emotional, social, and occupational aspects of the individual's life (Shariatirad et al., 2013; Volkow et al., 2011). Drug craving is considered by many researchers to be one of the main driving forces of drug-taking behaviors (Anton, 1999; Kassel and Shiffman, 1992; Robinson and Berridge, 1993). In spite of growing interest in craving which led to its consideration as one of the criteria for substance use disorders in DSM-5, there is a lack of consensuses about its definition (Sayette et al., 2000) and underlying causes (Drummond, 2001).

    • Cue-reactivity in experienced electronic cigarette users: Novel stimulus videos and a pilot fMRI study

      2016, Brain Research Bulletin
      Citation Excerpt :

      The study of craving has been a major theme in addiction research across drug types (Robinson and Berridge, 1993; Tiffany, 1990). In cigarette smoking more specifically, exposure to implicit and explicit cigarette cues produces neural, physiological, and cognitive responses that lead to increased desire to smoke (i.e., urges or craving) and subsequent smoking behavior (Abrams et al., 1988; Droungas et al., 1995; Due et al., 2002; Franklin et al., 2007; Janes et al., 2010; McBride et al., 2006; McClernon et al., 2005)–though craving and using do not always go hand-in-hand (Kassel and Shiffman, 1992; Shiffman, 2000; Tiffany, 1990). Given some of the unique characteristics of e-cigarette use compared to cigarette smoking noted above (e.g., more variable nicotine delivery, ability to use more frequently throughout the day, etc.), it is unclear to what extent craving, and more specifically cue-induced craving, play a role in e-cigarette use.

    • Smoking, food, and alcohol cues on subsequent behavior: A qualitative systematic review

      2015, Clinical Psychology Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      Moreover, target acquisition (e.g. breaking down and buying that pack of cigarettes) may occur dynamically over the course of several hours, potentially started by exposure to cues, but produced by slowly draining self-control resources until the person lacks the strength to resist. Many addictive processes are similar across substance and behavioral domains (Kassel & Shiffman, 1992; Köpetz et al., 2013; Pelchat, 2009; Tang, Fellows, Small, & Dagher, 2012), suggesting potential utility in examining the effect of cues on behavior across multiple targets. In the current review, we chose to evaluate behavioral responses to smoking, alcohol and food cues due to the ubquitiousness of these behaviors in Western society; 18% of US adults smoke, 52% of US adults report regular alcohol consumption, and two-thirds of adults are overweight or obese (Schiller, Lucas, & Peregoy, 2012).

    • Ambivalence about smoking and cue-elicited neural activity in quitting-motivated smokers faced with an opportunity to smoke

      2013, Addictive Behaviors
      Citation Excerpt :

      The authors proposed that overweight women may have avoided the high-calorie images because of their ambivalence about eating such food, and that this avoidance appeared to be relatively non-effortful (e.g., automatically shifting their gaze from the food cues; see also Werthmann et al., 2011). Given the phenomenological and neurobiological similarities between cue-elicited craving for food and addictive substances (Kassel & Shiffman, 1992; Volkow & Wise, 2005), highly ambivalent smokers might exhibit a pattern of neural responses to cigarette cues that is similar to those observed in overweight individuals presented with food stimuli. That is, compared to those with low levels of smoking ambivalence, participants with elevated smoking ambivalence may exhibit dampened activation of reward-related brain regions during cue exposure insofar as they approach such stimuli in a guarded manner; at the same time, smoking ambivalence may be unrelated to activity in conflict- and control-related regions if the processes that result in attenuation of reward-related responses are largely automatic or non-demanding.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text