Facial and vocal cues of deception and honesty

https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(79)90045-3Get rights and content

Abstract

Facial expressions and vocal cues (filtered speech) of honest and deceptive messages were examined in posed and spontaneous situations. The question of interest was the degree to which nonverbal cues transmit information about deception. Results indicated that (a) for both the facial and vocal channels, posing (as compared to spontaneous behavior) produced a higher level of communication accuracy; (b) facial expressions of deceptive (as compared to honest) messages were rated as less pleasant, while vocal expressions of deception were rated as less honest, less assertive, and less dominant, particularly in the posed condition; (c) the sender's ability to convey honesty was negatively correlated with his/her ability to convey deception, suggesting the existence of a demeanor bias—individual senders tend to appear and sound consistently honest (or dishonest) regardless of whether they deliver an honest or a deceptive message; (d) in the posing condition, the sender's abilities to convey honesty/deception via facial and vocal cues were positively and significantly correlated, whereas in the spontaneous condition they were not; and (e) senders whose full (unfiltered) speech indicated more involvement with their responses were judged as more honest from both their vocal (filtered speech) and facial cues, in both the honest and deceptive conditions.

References (29)

  • R. Buck

    Nonverbal communication of affect in children

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1975)
  • R. Buck et al.

    Sex, personality, and physiological variables in the communication of affect via facial expression

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1974)
  • R. Buck et al.

    Communication of affect through facial expressions in humans

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1972)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences

    (1977)
  • M.R. Cunningham

    Personality and the structure of the nonverbal communication of emotion

    Journal of Personality

    (1977)
  • B.M. DePaulo et al.

    Decoding discrepant nonverbal cues

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1978)
  • P. Ekman et al.

    Nonverbal leakage and clues to deception

    Psychiatry

    (1969)
  • P. Ekman et al.

    Detecting deception from the body or face

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1974)
  • P. Ekman et al.

    Body movement and voice pitch in deceptive interaction

    Semiotica

    (1976)
  • P.J. Fay et al.

    The ability to judge truth-telling, or lying, from the voice as transmitted over a public address system

    The Journal of General Psychology

    (1941)
  • R.S. Feldman

    Nonverbal disclosure of teacher deception and interpersonal affect

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1976)
  • A.A. Harrison et al.

    Cues to deception in an interview situation

    Social Psychology

    (1978)
  • R.E. Kraut

    Verbal and nonverbal cues in the perception of lying

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1978)
  • E.A. Levitt

    The relationship between abilities to express emotional meanings vocally and facially

  • Cited by (105)

    • Vocal characteristics of accuracy in eyewitness testimony

      2023, Speech Communication
      Citation Excerpt :

      In lie detection, that is, the distinction between truth (accurate) and lies (inaccurate), extensive research has been conducted to find if any vocal characteristics can differentiate truth-tellers from liars. Studies have looked at vocal characteristics such as; talking time (Mehrabian, 1971; Knapp et al., 1974; Todd-Mancillas and Kibler, 1979), speech rate (Anolli and Ciceri, 1997; DePaulo and DePaulo, 1989; Feeley and DeTurck, 1998), vocal immediacy (Kraut and Poe, 1980), vocal uncertainty (Kraut and Poe, 1980; Stiff and Miller, 1986), silent pauses (Anolli and Ciceri, 1997; Stiff and Miller, 1986), vocal pleasantness (Scherer et al., 1985; Zuckerman et al., 1979; Rockwell et al., 1997), vocal stress (O'Hair, Cody, 1987; O'Hair et al., 1990) and pitch (Villar et al., 2013; Streeter et al., 1977; Zuckerman et al., 1979; Rockwell et al., 1997). Although these individual studies generally find that the examined vocal characteristic in the respective study does predict accuracy, a meta-analysis of the field has revealed only small effects overall, which is a general issue in lie-detection research (DePaulo et al., 2003; see also Luke, 2019).

    • The sound of (in)sincerity

      2017, Journal of Pragmatics
      Citation Excerpt :

      Indeed, these insincere remarks are perceived as so innocuous that they are often grouped with compliments, courtesies (Ekman and O'Sullivan, 2006), and politeness behaviour (e.g., Talwar et al., 2007). In the literature on deception (unrestricted to prosocial lies), meta-analyses provide clues about the nonverbal indicators of insincerity during social interactions (DePaulo et al., 2003; Sporer and Schwandt, 2006; Zuckerman et al., 1979). Despite speakers’ attempts to hide that they are lying, auditory and visual cues that accompany the production of prosocial lies, such as changes in voice pitch or facial expressions, tend to “leak out” (Buller and Aune, 1987; Villar et al., 2013).

    • Neural correlates of inferring speaker sincerity from white lies: An event-related potential source localization study

      2014, Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      When the utterance content matches the speaker׳s true beliefs, emotions, and/or attitudes, it may be described as ‘sincere’; alternately, when the speaker conveys ideas that differ from his or her true beliefs, it may be described as insincere (and sometimes, a white lie). Researchers have characterized a number of nonverbal cues that tend to be associated with deception, such as subtle changes of expression, pitch of the voice, and body posture (Frank and Ekman 1997; Vrij, 1994; Zuckerman et al., 1979). Although much of the deception research focuses on high-stakes lying and associated forms of physiological arousal—which likely account for many of the verbal and non-verbal indicators of deception—the small lies which are part of normal daily interactions are not typically associated with these effects (DePaulo et al., 2003).

    • A Dynamic Model of Speech for the Social Sciences

      2021, American Political Science Review
    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This study was funded in part by Biomedical Research Support Grants, Division of Research Resources, National Institute of Health, to The Johns Hopkins University and to Harvard University, and by the Milton Fund of Harvard University.

    1

    Judith A. Hall was formerly Judith Hall Koivumaki.

    View full text