Elsevier

Physiology & Behavior

Volume 85, Issue 3, 30 June 2005, Pages 308-313
Physiology & Behavior

Oral sensations from iron and copper sulfate

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.04.018Get rights and content

Abstract

In previous work, retronasal smell was an important cue for perceiving metallic sensations from ferrous sulfate (FeSO4). The purpose of this study was to examine whether FeSO4 produced oral sensations as well as retronasal cues and whether astringency contributed to the oral sensations. In the first study, discrimination of FeSO4 from water was confirmed at concentrations at or above 5 mM without retronasal flow. A second study examined whether FeSO4 and CuSO4 could be detected on a non-gustatory region by tactile sensations. Astringency was perceived when the copper sulfate solution was applied to non-gustatory surfaces between the gum and the upper lip with or without retronasal cues. However, rated astringency for FeSO4 was less than that for CuSO4, and not significantly different from water. Other gustatory or tactile sensations from iron salts may contribute to their oral sensations and the possibility remains that at least one part of metallic sensation may be a metallic taste.

Introduction

Some divalent salts, such as those of iron, copper and zinc are described as having a metallic taste or flavor [1], [2]. This is particularly true of ferrous sulfate (FeSO4), which has been proposed as a prototypical metallic compound and as a reference standard in food sensory evaluation [3]. Because of its high bioavailability, ferrous sulfate is also commonly used in foods and dietary supplements as an iron-fortifying agent. However, the sensory profiles of divalent salts are complex, involving tastes, retronasal smell, and oral tactile properties such as astringency [2], [4], [5] rather than a simple taste or flavor. Hettinger et al. [6] proposed that the metallic flavor of ferrous sulfate is not a true gustatory quality but is likely to be due to olfaction. They observed that the frequencies of reports of metallic taste were decreased when the nose was occluded, eliminating retronasal smell. Subjects often have difficulty in identifying the locus of the stimulation when stimuli are introduced into the mouth [7]. Hollingworth and Poffenberger [8] and Murphy et al. [9] recognized that under uncertainty, people will report a retronasal olfactory sensation as a taste. These odor cues can be effectively eliminated by nasal occlusion [9]. Lawless et al. [1] confirmed Hettinger et al.'s results for FeSO4 but reported that retronasal metallic smell was less important for copper and zinc salts, which were characterized as more bitter and astringent. The solutions of FeSO4 had no smell when the headspace over the solutions was sniffed (orthonasal smell was absent), suggesting that the olfactory stimulus arises from rapidly generated lipid oxidation products in the mouth.

Very few chemical stimuli are truly monogustatory in quality, and FeSO4 is no exception. Several results indicate the possibility of sensing FeSO4 by tactile or gustatory mechanisms. Schiffman [10] measured detection thresholds for FeSO4 in the range of 0.147 mM when the nose was occluded with the type of clips used in spirometry (personal communication). This implies that detection was not by retronasal smell but by taste or tactile sensations. At the recognition threshold, subjects described the sensations from iron and zinc sulfate as “metallic.” In a duo–trio discrimination procedure, 0.5 mM FeSO4 was not discriminated from water with the nose occluded [1]. In contrast, discrimination of 0.3 mM CuSO4 from water was possible with and without nasal occlusion. However, in a same/different task, a greater frequency of subjects judged 1 mM FeSO4 “different” from water (as opposed to water against itself) even with their nose occluded [1]. If FeSO4 has oral qualities, it could involve taste or tactile sensations or both. Furthermore, they could vary with concentration.

Oral astringency is a quality sometimes confused with gustatory sensations. In her history of research on taste, Bartoshuk [11] described how Aristotle, and Galen after him, considered astringency as a ‘taste’, along with the qualities of ‘harshness’ and ‘pungency’. In a recent paper, Green [12] discussed the psychophysical, chemical and physiological bases of astringency and concluded that oral astringency is of tactile origin and is caused principally by the precipitation of salivary mucoproteins, which impairs the natural lubrication of oral surfaces. Breslin et al. [5] asked subjects to rate the intensity of astringency after they applied an alum solution to the non-gustatory surface between the upper lip and gums. Oral astringency was evoked on this non-gustatory region, confirming it's tactile nature. Whether FeSO4 and CuSO4 would have a similar effect is not known.

The purpose of this study was to improve our understanding of the types of sensations generated by FeSO4 and CuSO4 that are often described as having metallic taste or flavor as an extension of the previous study [1]. In particular, the possibilities of oral and tactile sensations from those stimuli were tested. The first study examined whether FeSO4 produced oral sensations by determining whether it could be discriminated from water at different concentrations when the nose was occluded. CuSO4 was not included in the first experiment below because oral discrimination with the nose closed was previously demonstrated [1]. In a second study, the potential contribution of astringency to the oral perception of FeSO4 and CuSO4 was examined using Breslin et al.'s [5] procedure of stimulating non-gustatory areas between the gum and lip. If astringency is perceived in the non-gustatory area, this would support a tactile cue to explain the discrimination of ferrous salts from water. If not, then the possibility remains of a true gustatory cue in the perception of ferrous sulfate and that such sensations contribute to at least part of its “metallic taste”.

Section snippets

Subjects

Thirty-six healthy subjects (13 M and 23 F; age 20–52, average 26 years) from the Cornell University community in Ithaca, NY volunteered to participate. They reported no taste problems. Subjects were untrained and naïve to the hypotheses of the study. They were requested not to eat or drink for at least 1 h before each test. Informed consent was given before the test and compensation was provided at the conclusion. Research protocols were approved by the University Committee on Human Subjects.

Stimuli

Experiment 2

Experiment 1 demonstrated that iron sulfate produced oral sensations without retronsal smell at concentrations of 5 mM or higher. The possibilities remain that subjects could discriminate iron and copper solutions from water by tactile sensations or by a true gustatory quality. A second experiment was designed to determine whether the perception of iron and copper salts depend on astringency cues by applying the solutions to a non-gustatory area between the gum and upper lip. The same strategy,

General discussion

Although the perception of metallic compounds is not well understood, the term ‘metallic taste’ is commonly used to describe many sensations evoked by metallic compounds. In food science, metallic taste has been characterized as ‘an astringent, metallic sensation, which is similar to that observed when an iron nail or metal foil is placed in the mouth’ [19]. Metal salts are often described as having metallic taste [20], [21], [22], [23], metallic off-flavor [24], [25], [26], after-taste [27],

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Kathryn W. Chapman for technical assistance. Supported by NIH RO1-DC-006223 to HL.

References (39)

  • Yang HH, Lawless HT. Descriptive analysis of divalent salts. J Sens Stud in...
  • G.V. Civille et al.

    Aroma and flavor lexicon for sensory evaluation

    (1996)
  • R.S.J. Keast

    The effect of zinc on human taste perception

    J Food Sci

    (2003)
  • P.A.S. Breslin et al.

    Psychophysical evidence that oral astringency is a tactile sensation

    Chem Senses

    (1993)
  • T.P. Hettinger et al.

    Role of olfaction in perception of non-traditional ‘taste’ stimuli

    Chem Senses

    (1990)
  • H.L. Hollingworth et al.

    The sense of taste

    (1917)
  • C. Murphy et al.

    Mutual action of taste and olfaction

    Sens Process

    (1977)
  • S.S. Schiffmann

    Taste quality and neural coding: implications from psychophysics and neurophysiology

    Phys Behav

    (2000)
  • L.M. Bartoshuk

    History of research on taste

  • Cited by (39)

    • The impacts of water compositions on sensory properties of foods and beverages cannot be underestimated

      2018, Food Research International
      Citation Excerpt :

      Metals such as iron, copper, magnesium, zinc, and manganese impact the taste, odor and the mouthfeel of water. Older research studies focused more on the taste threshold of metals, but more recent studies showed the metallic sensation could be perceived through gustatory, olfactory and trigeminal (chemesthetic) sensations (Lawless, Stevens, Chapman, & Kurtz, 2005a; Lim & Lawless, 2005; Omur-Ozbek & Dietrich, 2011; Yang & Lawless, 2006a, 2006b). Therefore, it is more appropriate to use the term “sensation” rather than “taste” or “flavor” for the metallic perception.

    • Tasting spoons: Assessing how the material of a spoon affects the taste of the food

      2012, Food Quality and Preference
      Citation Excerpt :

      The subsequent success of this material resulted in a radical reduction in the range of materials used for cutlery, so that nowadays, stainless steel is by far the most commonly used material for cutlery. The tastes (or sensations) of metals and their impact on food perception and quality judgments have been studied extensively (e.g., Epke, McClure, & Lawless, 2009; Hettinger, Myers, & Frank, 1990; Hoehl, Schoenberger, & Busch-Stockfisch, 2010; Keast, 2003; Lawless, Stevens, Chapman, & Kurtz, 2005; Lawless et al., 2004; Lim & Lawless, 2005; Schiffman, 2000; Stevens, Smith, & Lawless, 2006). However, the majority of these studies were performed on aqueous solutions, with metal salts that are usually incorporated in fortified foods, food supplements, and drinks, and hence easily soluble.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text