The influence of television and video game use on attention and school problems: A multivariate analysis with other risk factors controlled

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.11.010Get rights and content

Abstract

Background

Research on youth mental health has increasingly indicated the importance of multivariate analyses of multiple risk factors for negative outcomes. Television and video game use have often been posited as potential contributors to attention problems, but previous studies have not always been well-controlled or used well-validated outcome measures. The current study examines the multivariate nature of risk factors for attention problems symptomatic of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and poor school performance.

Method

A predominantly Hispanic population of 603 children (ages 10–14) and their parents/guardians responded to multiple behavioral measures. Outcome measures included parent and child reported attention problem behaviors on the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) as well as poor school performance as measured by grade point average (GPA).

Results

Results found that internal factors such as male gender, antisocial traits, family environment and anxiety best predicted attention problems. School performance was best predicted by family income. Television and video game use, whether total time spent using, or exposure to violent content specifically, did not predict attention problems or GPA.

Interpretation

Television and video game use do not appear to be significant predictors of childhood attention problems. Intervention and prevention efforts may be better spent on other risk factors.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants include 603 youth from a small city in South Texas (see Ferguson et al., 2009). Youth included in the present study ranged in age from 10-14, with a mean age of 12.35 (SD = 1.34). The majority of youth in the present study were Hispanic in ethnicity (96.8%). This sample was approximately equal in numbers of males (n = 309, 51.2%) and females (n = 294, 48.8%). The current sample is a general sample of youth, not an at-risk sample. In regards to ethnicity, income, country of birth

Results

Results for bivariate correlations between measures are presented in Table 2. Table 3 presents the standardized regression coefficients and 95% confidence intervals (for significant coefficients) for all variables in the three regression equations. For attention problems as reported by the child significant correlates included mental health problems such as depression (β = .18), anxiety (β = .32) and antisocial traits (β = .17), as well as negative relations with adults (β = .14) and

Discussion

Results from the current study suggest that attention problems are predicted most accurately by intrinsic factors such as anxiety, antisocial traits and male gender, as well as by delinquent peers and negative associations with adults. Anxiety symptoms, in fact, appear to be one of the strongest and most consistent risk factors for attention problems. This may be because the rumination symptoms of anxiety interfere with attention. For GPA, it appears that wealthier families are better able to

Role of funding source

No external funding source. All funding is through state of Texas via local university research grants (less than $10000).

Contributors

Christopher J. Ferguson, Ph.D.

Conflict of interest

None declared.

Acknowledgement

None.

References (30)

  • J. Rucklidge

    Gender differences in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

    Psychiatric Clinics of North America

    (2010)
  • T.M. Achenbach et al.

    Manual for ASEBA school-age forms & profiles

    (2001)
  • M. Alqahtani

    The comorbidity of ADHD in the general population of Saudi Arabian school-age children

    Journal of Attention Disorders

    (2010)
  • D.R. Anderson et al.

    The effects of TV program pacing on the behavior of preschool children

    AV Communication Review

    (1977)
  • R. Barkley et al.

    Consensus statement on ADHD

    European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

    (2002)
  • K. Beaver et al.

    Delinquent peer group formation: evidence of a gene x environment correlation

    The Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human Development

    (2008)
  • J. Biederman et al.

    Family-environment risk factors for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a test of Rutter’s indicators of adversity

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1995)
  • J. Biederman et al.

    The longitudinal course of comorbid oppositional defiant disorder in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Findings from a controlled 5-year prospective longitudinal follow-up study

    Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics

    (December 2008)
  • T. Ceranoglu

    Video games in psychotherapy

    Review of General Psychology

    (2010)
  • D.A. Christakis et al.

    Early television exposure and subsequent attentional problems in children

    Pediatrics

    (2004)
  • K. Durkin

    Videogames and young people with developmental disorders

    Review of General Psychology

    (2010)
  • M. Dye et al.

    Increasing speed of processing with action video games

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (2009)
  • C.J. Ferguson et al.

    Violent video games and aggression: Causal relationship or byproduct of family violence and intrinsic violence motivation?

    Criminal Justice and Behavior

    (2008)
  • C. Ferguson et al.

    A multivariate analysis of youth violence and aggression: the influence of family, peers, depression, and media violence

    The Journal of Pediatrics

    (2009)
  • C. Ferguson

    Blazing angels or resident evil? Can violent video games be a force for good?

    Review of General Psychology

    (2010)
  • Cited by (71)

    • Screen exposure exacerbates ADHD symptoms indirectly through increased sleep disturbance

      2021, Sleep Medicine
      Citation Excerpt :

      Furthermore, evening screen exposure is associated with a disruption in daytime attention/behaviors in children aged 6 to 16 through its mediated effect on exacerbating sleep disturbance. This mediated effect could explain the absence of correlation between screen exposure and ADHD symptoms observed in several other correlational studies [36–39], because they did not control sleep perturbation (SDSC scores), nor the time of day of screen exposure (morning, afternoon, evening). Only longitudinal studies were able to observe the deleterious effect of screen exposure on ADHD symptoms in children [35–37].

    • An empirical study on gender, video game play, academic success and complex problem solving skills

      2018, Computers and Education
      Citation Excerpt :

      Supporting this, several studies have reported a negative relationship between video game play and academic success (Anand, 2007; Choo et al., 2010; Gentile et al., 2004; Weaver, Kim, Metzer, & Szendrey, 2013). On the other hand, there are also studies that found no relationship between video gaming time and academic success (Brunborg et al., 2014; Drummond & Sauer, 2014; Ferguson, 2011; Sahin et al., 2016). The current findings are in favor of the latter.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text