Skip to main content
Log in

Why is Intelligence Negatively Associated with Religiousness?

  • Theoretical Article
  • Published:
Evolutionary Psychological Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We present three models which attempt to explain the robust negative association between religion and intelligence: the Irrationality of Religion Model, the Cultural Mediation Hypothesis, and the Savanna-IQ Interaction Hypothesis. We highlight problems with each of them and propose that the negative religion-IQ nexus can be understood through substantially revising the Savanna-IQ Interaction Hypothesis. We argue that religion should be regarded as an evolved domain or instinct. Intelligence, by contrast, involves rising above our instincts. It follows that an inclination toward the non-instinctive will thus be an aspect of intelligence because it will help us to solve problems. Thus, intelligence will involve being attracted to evolutionary mismatch, to that which we would not be instinctively evolved to be attracted to. It is this, we argue, that is behind the negative religion-intelligence nexus. We respond to potential criticisms of our model and we examine how this model can be further tested.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The lack of significance in pre-college samples may be because those who are younger have been more exposed to atheistic ideologies, especially at an earlier age. In addition, it may be due to a restriction of range caused by the fact that, in the samples in question, belief in God was extremely high among those under the age of around 14. For example, in Turner (1980), who used a Northern Irish sample, the nexus was significant among 14-, 15-, and 16-year-olds, but not among those who were younger. The finding of non-significance in the six all-male samples is due to sampling anomalies. Three of the studies (two from the USA and one from Brazil) used church membership/attendance rather than religious belief (Bender 1968; Kosa and Schommer 1961 and Szobot et al. 2007). This is problematic as a measure of religious belief because social conformity and virtue signaling may play a role in church attendance especially in 1960s USA and strongly Catholic Brazil. In addition, the correlation with church attendance is typically half that of belief and samples were small, ranging from 96 to 361. Feather (1967) uses a sample of only 40 and they are “pro-religion” (30) and “anti-religion” (10), which is not the same as “theist” and “atheist.” Finally, Turner (1980) uses an all-male pre-college sample. As discussed, his finding would seem to reflect a restriction of range due to overwhelming religious belief among pre-pubescent Northern Irish children. Webster and Duffy’s finding of non-significance when correlating national IQ with national religiousness seems to reflect their controls being too strict. They control for life quality index and whether countries are close to each other. By doing this, they are taking away a large share of the variance.

  2. Though this is the case in general, there is some evidence that very fervent atheists can become zealous in belief in “scientism” (the absolute and dogmatic belief in science) when under intense stress (Farias 2013). It could be argued that we are evolved to desire pattern and certainty (see Boyer 2001) and that this instinct can manifest itself in scientism with a minority of people. It has been argued that there are parallels between aspects of religion—fervent acceptance of dogma, for example—and scientism (see Jenkins 2009).

  3. For example, a cult surrounding Padre Pio (1887–1968) took off after his death and is especially followed in southern Italy, where he came from (Hauschild 2011, p.15). He was beatified in 1999 and made a saint in 2002. In Catholic theology, you can pray to anyone in Heaven you like and ask them to pray to God for you (intercede). The first stage of canonization is to be declared a “servant of God.” Stage two is to be declared “heroic in virtue” (Venerable). This means you might already be in Heaven. Stage three is to be beatified as “Blessed,” meaning you have definitely entered Heaven (having served your time in Purgatory), and people can pray to you, certain that you can intercede on their behalf. To be beatified, your intercession must have led to one miracle or you must have been martyred. Finally, if your intercession leads to two miracles, then you are made a saint, unless you are a martyr in which case only one miracle is needed.

References

  • BaHammam, A. (2011). Sleep from an Islamic perspective. Annals of Thoracic Medicine, 6, 187–192.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J. (2004). Why would anyone believe in god? Walnut Creek: AltaMira Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bender, I. (1968). A longitudinal study of church attenders and non-attenders. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 7, 230–237. doi:10.2307/1384630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blume, M. (2009). The reproductive benefits of religious affiliation. In E. Voland & W. Schiefenhövel (Eds.), The biological evolution of religious mind and behavior. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: the human instincts that fashion gods, spirits and ancestors. London: William Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brierley, P. (2010). UK Christianity: 2005–2015. London: Christian Research Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Browne, T. (1672). Pseudoxia epidemica: or enquiries into the very many received tenets and commonly presumed truths. London: Printed for Edward Dod and are to be Sould (sic.) by Andrew Crook.

  • Cofnas, N. (2012). Reptiles with a conscience: the co-evolution of religious and moral doctrine. London: Ulster Institute for Social Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cofnas, N. (2016). A teleofunctional account of everyday evolutionary mismatch. Biology and Philosophy, 31, 507–525.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cornelis, I., Van Heil, A., Roets, A., & Kassowska, M. (2008). Age differences in conservatism: evidence on the mediating effects of personality and cognitive style. Journal of Personality, 77, 51–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeYoung, C., Peterson, J., & Higgins, D. (2005). Sources of openness/intellect: cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. Journal of Personality, 73, 825–858.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunkel, C., Reeve, C., Woodley of Menie, M. A., & Van der Linden, D. (2015). A comparative study of the general factor of personality in Jewish and non-Jewish populations. Personality and Individual Differences, 78, 63–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durrant, R., & Ellis, B. (2003). Evolutionary psychology. In M. Gallagher & R. Nelson (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: volume I: biological psychology. Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, E. (2013a). The cultural mediation hypotheses: a critical examination. Intelligence, 41, 321–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, E. (2013b). The Savanna-IQ Interaction hypothesis: a critical examination of the comprehensive case presented in Kanazawa’s The Intelligence Paradox. Intelligence, 41, 607–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, E. (2014). Religion and intelligence: an evolutionary analysis. London: Ulster Institute for Social Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, E., & Charlton, B. (2015). The genius famine: why we need geniuses, why they are dying out and why we must rescue them. Buckingham: Buckingham University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, E., & Lynn, R. (2014). Intelligence and religious and political differences among the US academic elite. Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on Religion, 10, 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, E., & Van der Linden, D. (2015). Who are the “clever sillies”? The intelligence, personality, and motives of clever silly originators and those who follow them. Intelligence, 49, 57–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, E., Van der Linden, D., & Lynn, R. (2016a). The negative Flynn effect: a systematic literature review. Intelligence, 59, 163–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, E., Madison, G., & Lynn, R. (2016b). Demographic, economic, and genetic factors related to national differences in ethnocentric attitudes. Personality and Individual Differences, 101, 137–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N. (2012). Temperamental effortful control (self-regulation). Encyclopedia on early childhood development, http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/temperament/according-experts/temperamental-effortful-control-self-regulation.

  • Ellis, L., Hoskin, A., Dutton, E., & Nyborg, H. (2017). The future of secularism: a biologically informed theory supplemented with cross-cultural evidence. Evolutionary Psychological Science. doi:10.1007/s40806-017-0090-z.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farias, M. (2013). The psychology of atheism. In S. Bullivant & M. Ruse (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of atheism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feather, N. T. (1967). Evaluation of religious and neutral arguments in religious and atheist student groups. Australian Journal of Psychology, 19, 3–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Figueredo, A., Vasquez, G., Brumbach, B., et al. (2006). Consilience and life history theory: from genes to brain to reproductive strategy. Developmental Review, 26, 243–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, J. (2012). Are we getting smarter? Rising IQ in the twenty-first century. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gaskill, M. (2003). Crime and mentalities in early modern England. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilkey, C. K. (1924). Religion among American students. Journal of Religion, 1, 4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodey, C. F. (2011). A history of intelligence and “intellectual disability”: the shaping of psychology in early modern Europe. Farnham: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, B., & Marshall, P. (Eds.). (2000). The place of the dead: death and remembrance in late medieval and early modern Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammond, R., & Axelrod, R. (2006). The evolution of ethnocentric behaviour. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 50, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauschild, T. (2011). Power and Magic in Italy. Oxford: Berghahn Books.

  • Howells, T. (1928). A comparative study of those who accept as against those who reject religious authority. University of Iowa Studies in Character, 2, 3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, W. (2015). The English deists: studies in early in Enlightenment. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1896/1956). The will to believe and other essays in popular philosophy. New York: Dover Publications.

  • James, W. (1907). Pragmatism: a new name for some old ways of thinking. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, T. (2009). Faith and the scientific mind/faith in the scientific mind: the implicit religion of science in contemporary Britain. Implicit Religion, 12, 3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. (1998). The g factor: the science of mental ability. Westport: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, J. (2013). Pragmatic arguments and belief in God. Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy, https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/pragmatic-belief-god/#PraArg.

  • Kanazawa, S. (2012). The intelligence paradox: why the intelligent choice isn’t always the smart one. Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay, A. C., Shepherd, S., Blatz, C. W., et al. (2010). For god (or) country: the hydraulic relation between government instability and belief in religious sources of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 725–739.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • King, C. (2008). Faustus and the Promise of the New Science, c. 1580-1730: From the Chapbooks to Harlequin Faustus. Aldershot: Ashgate.

  • Koenig, L., McGue, M., Krueger, R., & Bouchard, T. (2005). Genetic and environmental influences on religiousness: findings for retrospective and current religiousness ratings. Journal of Personality, 73, 471–478.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kosa, J., & Schommer, C. (1961). Religious participation, religious knowledge, and scholastic aptitude: an empirical study. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 1, 88–97. doi:10.2307/1385179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R., & Witham, L. (1998). Leading scientists still reject god. Nature, 394, 313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, G., Ritchie, S., & Bates, C. (2011). The relationship between intelligence and multiple domains of religious belief: evidence from a large US adult sample. Intelligence, 39, 468–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, R. (2011). Dysgenics: genetic deterioration in modern populations. London: Ulster Institute for Social Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2012). Intelligence: a unifying construct for the social sciences. London: Ulster Institute for Social Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, K. (2010). Evolution as a dual processing theory of culture: applications to moral idealism and political philosophy. Politics and Culture, 1: http://politicsandculture.org/2010/04/29/evolution-and-a-dual-processing-theory-of-culture-applications-to-moral-idealism-and-political-philosophy/.

  • Maroney, E. (2006). Religious syncretism. London: SCM Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, J. (2006). John Locke, toleration and early Enlightenment culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meisenberg, G. (2007). In God’s image: the natural history of intelligence and ethics. Kibworth: Book Guild Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meisenberg, G., Rindermann, H., Patel, H., & Woodley, M. (2012). Is it smart to believe in god? The relationship of religiosity with education and intelligence. Temas em Psicologia, 20, 101–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mill, J. S. (1909). Autobiography. London: P. F. Collier and Son.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murashko, A. (2013). Air Force Republishes Chaplain’s ‘No Atheists in Foxholes’ Article to Base Website. Christian Post, http://www.christianpost.com/news/air-force-republishes-chaplains-no-atheists-in-foxholes-article-to-base-website-102226/.

  • Newberg, A., D’Aquili, E., & Rause, V. (2002). Why god won’t go away: brain science and the biology of belief. New York: Ballantine Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious pro-sociality. Science, 322, 58–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nyborg, H. (2009). The intelligence-religiosity nexus: a representative study of white adolescent Americans. Intelligence, 37, 81–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ritchie, S., Gow, A., & Deary, I. (2014). Religiosity is negatively associated with later-life intelligence, but not with age-related cognitive decline. Intelligence, 46, 9–17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rushton, J. P. (2000). Race, evolution and behavior: a life history perspective, 3rd special edition. Port Huron: Charles Darwin Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, B. (1952). Is there a God? In: The collected papers of Bertrand Russell, Vol. 11: last philosophical testament, 1943–68. London: Routledge.

  • Saroglou, V. (2002). Religion and the five factors of personality: a meta-analytic review. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 15–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scruton, R. (2000). Modern culture. London: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedgwick, M. (2009). Against the modern world: traditionalism and the secret intellectual history of the twentieth century. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snobelen, S. (1999). Isaac Newton, heretic: the strategies of a Nicodemite. British Journal for the History of Science, 32, 381–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K., & West, R. (2008). On the failure of intelligence to predict myside bias and one-sided bias. Thinking & Reasoning, 14, 129–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steimer, T. (2002). The biology of fear- and anxiety-related behaviors. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 4, 231–249.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Swenson, D. (2008). Religion and family links: Neofunctionalist reflections. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Symonds, J. (1902). Studies of the Greek poets. London: A. & C. Black.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szobot, C. M., Rohde, L. A., Bukstein, O., Molina, B. S. G., Martins, C., Ruaro, P., & Pechansky, F. (2007). Is attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder associated with illicit substance use disorders in male adolescents? A community-based case-control study. Addiction, 102, 1122–1130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turchin, P. (2007). War and peace and war: the rise and fall of empires. New York: Plume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, E. (1980). General cognitive ability and religious attitudes in two school systems. British Journal of Religious Education, 2, 136–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Linden, D., Pekaar, K., Bakker, A., Schermer, J., Vernon, P., Dunkel, C., & Petrides, K. (2016). Overlap between the general factor of personality and emotional intelligence: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. doi:10.1037/bul0000078.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verhage, F. (1964). Intelligence and religious persuasion. Nederlands tijdschift voor de psychologie en haar grensgebieden, 19, 247–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, R. (1994). The rest is silence: death as annihilation in the English renaissance. Los Angeles: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster, G., & Duffy, R. (2016). Losing faith in the intelligence-religiosity link: new evidence for a decline effect, spatial dependence, and mediation by education and life quality. Intelligence, 55, 15–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West, R., Meserve, R., & Stanovich, K. (2012). Cognitive sophistication does not attenuate the bias blind spot. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 506–519.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitmarsh, T. (2016). Battling the gods: atheism in the ancient world. London: Faber & Faber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: the unity of knowledge. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodley, M. A. (2010). Are high-IQ individuals deficient in common sense? A critical examination of the ‘clever sillies’ hypothesis. Intelligence, 38, 471–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodley of Menie, M. A., & Dunkel, C. (2015). Beyond the cultural mediation hypothesis: a reply to Dutton (2013). Intelligence, 49, 186–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodley of Menie, M. A., & Fernandes, H. (2015). Do opposing secular trends on backwards and forwards digit span evidence the co-occurrence model? A comment on Gignac (2015). Intelligence, 50, 125–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodley, M. A., te Nijenhuis, J., & Murphy, R. (2013). Were the Victorians cleverer than us?: The decline in general intelligence estimated from a meta-analysis of the slowing of simple reaction time. Intelligence, 41, 843–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, L. (Ed.). (2016). Rational choice theory and religion: a summary and assessment. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M., Silverman, J., & Hall, J. A. (2013). The relation between intelligence and religiosity: a meta-analysis and some proposed explanations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 325–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Michael Woodley of Menie and Curtis Dunkel for their helpful comments on this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Edward Dutton.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dutton, E., Van der Linden, D. Why is Intelligence Negatively Associated with Religiousness?. Evolutionary Psychological Science 3, 392–403 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-017-0101-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-017-0101-0

Keywords

Navigation