Introduction

Quality of life (QoL) is a multi-factorial construct (Argyle, 1999; Hammer et al., 2019; Newman et al., 2013) seen as either health-related (Bakas et al., 2012), linked with the status/availability of resources (Hagerty & Veenhoven, 2003; Sőrés & Pető, 2015) or a more personal phenomenon mediated through the subjective perception of objective factors such as relationships, work, money, health or leisure (Brannan et al., 2012; Diener et al., 2017; Diener et al., 1999; Steptoe et al., 2015). It commonly refers to a person’s perceptions, thoughts and feelings about such life conditions, and reactions to those conditions (Malkoç, 2011; The World Health Organisation, 2015).

QoL is typically distinguished from subjective well-being (SWB), sometimes colloquially known as ‘happiness’, which more refers to a global assessment of a person’s life and typically includes both positive and negative affect and cognitive evaluations of life satisfaction (Diener et al., 2013; Luhmann et al., 2012; Lyubomirsky, 2013). However, these terms are often used interchangeably in research and public policy (Peasgood et al., 2019) even though they are usually underpinned by different theory and measurement fields (Dolan et al., 2016).

For example, Tay, Kuykendall and Diener’s (Diener et al., 2013) observation that “SWB can also be categorized by different life domains such as marriage, leisure, or job” (p.840) could easily have applied to quality of life. In response to this, Skevington and Böhnke (2018) make a strong case for a new Life Quality and Well-being model that integrates the overlapping and exclusive dimensions of both constructs. Whilst the identification of predictors and behaviours that might help individuals report higher quality of life and well-being is of primary research interest (Lachman et al., 2015; Nickel et al., 2019), there is comparatively less data about their enhancement (Sitbon et al., 2019) or, indeed, programs that specifically address quality of life and well-being (Corten et al., 1994; Cummins, 2003; Sergeant & Mongrain, 2015).

In recent years, however, there has been growing research into the link between the field of mindfulness and well-being (Brewer et al., 2011; Hanley et al., 2014; Kreplin et al., 2018; Kudesia & Nyima, 2014; Schoenberg & Vago, 2018; Sedlmeier et al., 2012). Mindfulness training offers a range of concentration, awareness and loving kindness methods, though not always clearly distinguished (Van Dam et al., 2010), that focus on developing an individual’s conscious awareness of thoughts, emotions (Kabat-Zinn, 2011) and the cultivation of compassion (Kirby et al., 2017).

By being assisted to observe these processes more objectively, individuals experience greater choice to engage in or disengage from these thoughts and emotions to optimise well-being (Feldman et al., 2010; Lindahl & Britton, 2019; Segal et al., 2002). Through the development of skills such as objectivity and greater present moment awareness, outcomes of mindfulness training include reduced rumination, the activation of “task-positive” brain regions related to life skills such as conflict monitoring and cognitive control (Anderson et al., 2007; Vago & Nakamura, 2011), and increased well-being (Compson, 2018; Malinowski, 2013; Tacon et al., 2004).

Similarly, in the fields of performance and goals theory (Brunstein, 1993; Sheldon et al., 2004) there has also been applicable research for the improvement of perceived quality of life (Threadgill & Gable, 2018). For example, the attainment of personally relevant, intrinsic and meaningful goals (Emmons, 2003; Lapierre et al., 2007; Locke & Schattke, 2018;) and the process of achieving or moving toward goals has been associated with positive affect and well-being (Plemmons & Weiss, 2013) across the life span (López Ulloa et al., 2013). For example, people who follow intrinsically derived goals are more able to integrate the diverse areas of their lives, and their perception of goal success through reducing the gap between their expectations and outcomes (achievement-aspiration gap), increases the perception of quality of life (Land et al., 2011).

The goal of this research was to design a program that targeted both the subjective components of well-being and the objective components of quality of life. As empirical reviews of mindfulness training have found mindfulness to be effective in improving mental processes (Ortner & Zelazo, 2014; Thomas et al., 2016) relevant to perceived well-being and quality of life (Keng et al., 2011), mindfulness appeared to be a suitable inclusion in a program that taught the cognitive skill sets foundational to the development and maintenance of subjective well-being.

There were some distinct advantages that came with the choice of mindfulness, one specifically relevant to the formulation of perceived quality of life, that mindfulness may be able to reduce the “achievement-aspiration gap” by assisting someone to “want what one has,” which in turn leads to an increase in the perception of quality of life (Brown et al., 2009). Furthermore, there was some early precedent with Smith, Compton and West’s (1995) finding that the addition of meditation to Fordyce’s (1983, 1988) earlier happiness and quality of life program improved its efficacy. Finally, the strong presence of mindfulness in the intervention literature, and the well documented findings of its impact upon relevant areas such as positive self-esteem (Robins et al., 2001), internal locus of control (Strickland, 2016), the ability to modulate behaviour (Raffone & Srinivasan, 2009), and interpersonal skills (Shonin et al., 2015) seemed sufficient reason for choosing mindfulness as a key focus of the intervention.

Similarly, as the research indicated that goal setting is an effective method to heighten achievement in the major life domains (Grouzet et al., 2005; Seijts et al., 2013), this methodology was included in the intervention to address the objective components of quality of life. In constructing the intervention, this research aimed to address a gap in the quality of life and subjective well-being literature, as much of current research focuses on predictors and assessment than on intervention design. By combining mindfulness to address the subjective components of well-being, and goal setting for improvement of the objective components of perceived quality of life, this research sought to provide a novel and more comprehensive approach to enhancing the self-report of happiness.

Furthermore, a perceived gap in the mindfulness intervention literature was also addressed. It was considered that there may be a greater focus on the effect of mindfulness upon subjective processes or health-related outcomes, rather than creating change in objective life domains identified in the quality of life and subjective well-being literature. By integrating findings from the QoL field there was an opportunity to examine the efficacy of mindfulness to impact identified quality of life domains.

The five life domains chosen to work on that were identified as having an impact on the perception of quality of life were: relationships, work, money, health and leisure. In brief, the multi-axial domain of relationships satisfaction is one of the major predictors of reported well-being (Carr et al., 2014; Demir & Weitekamp, 2007; Proulx et al., 2007) and can include intimate relationships, family, friends and colleagues. It is founded on the perception of connection, bonding and satisfying interaction with others (Gere & Schimmack, 2011) and can act as a buffer for stress and social support (Brannan et al., 2012), help achieve positive health outcomes (Taylor, 2010), and increased well-being (Tan & Tay, 2017).

Work also plays a significant role in quality of life (Van Katwyk et al., 2000) and includes work conditions (Cranny et al., 1992), supportive management (Monnot & Beehr, 2014), satisfaction of psychological needs at work (Parfyonova et al., 2019), and vocational satisfaction being linked to greater life satisfaction (Shimazu et al., 2015). Money appears to play a small but critical role in the perception of quality of life (Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2009) with financial goal setting having positive outcomes on health and wellbeing (Brackertz, 2013) and life satisfaction associated with gross domestic product (Hagerty & Veenhoven, 2003). However, a negative effect can also be experienced if material goals are prized more than other values (Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2009), and financial problems have also been found to be a predictor of depression (Huppert & So, 2011).

Health-related quality of life research is a burgeoning area (Brazier & Tsuchiya, 2015; Cummins et al., 2004; Salvador-Carulla et al., 2014) and involves a reciprocal relationship between health status and well-being, with affect correlating with symptom checklists (Krampen, 1999), physical functioning, pain and general health (Czekierda et al., 2019; Karimi & Brazier, 2016). The relationship also appears to be bidirectional, with people that present with heart disease, arthritis, and lung disease having increased levels of depressed mood and impaired hedonic and eudemonic wellbeing (Boehm & Kubzansky, 2012; Steptoe et al., 2015).

Finally, leisure has a small but significant association with mental health (Lloyd & Auld, 2002; Tinsley & Eldredge, 1995), and life satisfaction (Rodríguez et al., 2007). Again, a multidimensional construct, it can also act as a buffer for stress through the pursuit of rewarding activities (Csíkszentmihályi, 2014) that transcend daily concerns (Sonnentag & Niessen, 2008). Through the effort-recovery process (Tay et al., 2014) it is seen that resilience is built, which plays a protective role in the maintenance of well-being (Denovan & Macaskill, 2016).

The trial design chosen was a three-arm randomised controlled trial (RCT) comparing two experimental treatments [graduated learning six week format (6 weeks x 2 hours), a two day intensive format (2 days x 6 hours) and control], adhering to the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) (Schulz, 2010). The rationale for choosing a multi-arm randomized clinical trial that evaluated more than one new intervention concurrently included: the increased probability of finding an effective intervention; the increase of efficiency (lower costs due to better use of resources); sharing of a control reduces total required sample size compared to separate two-arm trials; and offering participants more possibilities of being allocated to another intervention option could result in a greater number of people enrolling (Juszczak et al., 2019).

In study 1, the first aim was to test the hypothesis that the combined intervention itself (six week and two day version) was more effective at improving self-report of quality of life and subjective well-being in comparison to a waitlist control group at post-test and follow up. The second aim, in response to research into teaching frameworks that create "learning that lasts" (Lee, 2001), explored which training formats are most effective for complex learning (Van Merriënboer & Kirschner, 2017). As there is some indication that there is little or no difference in achievement between compressed and traditional formats (Arrey, 2005; Wlodkowski & Westover, 1999), the second aim of this study was to provide further data (Halldorsdottir & Ollendick, 2016) on whether the intervention design (either six week or two day version) influenced gains in well-being. This was regarded as providing useful information on the efficacy of each intervention delivery option.

In study 2, the aim was to test the hypothesis that the full Mindfulness-based Quality of Life and Well-Being Program (MQW) would be more effective at improving self-report of quality of life and subjective well-being than teaching mindfulness alone. To test this, the follow up study was conducted comparing the full 6-week version of the MQW program to the mindfulness section of the program. This was designed by delivering the mindfulness content of the first 2-weeks of the full program across 6 weeks and increasing the practise time of the exercises.

Study 1

Method

Program Development

Interventions which give participants both information and the skills to positively change their behaviours (Fishbach & Hofmann, 2015; Sin & Lyubomirsky, 2009) result in increased quality of life (Bonow et al., 2012; Folette et al., 2001). As such, the Mindfulness-based Quality of Life and Well-Being Program (MQW) was made up of both psychoeducation and interactive exercises, and participants were invited to apply the program to their life circumstances so as to consolidate treatment gains (Brown et al., 2017).

The program delivery sequence of first teaching theory (both mindfulness and quality of life/well-being) before teaching method drew from quality of life theory and from previous mindfulness programs (Kabat-Zinn, 1990; Shapiro et al., 2007) that introduced participants to general mindfulness theory and principles, before teaching techniques and exercises. It also mapped the order of traditional Buddhist mindfulness training structures made up of study, contemplation and meditation (Kudesia & Nyima, 2014).

This latter sequenced approach to mindfulness begins with theory (study) before moving to its application in daily life (contemplation). Methods (meditation) to build attentional regulation skills and assist the decrease of discursive cognition are added, before moving to deconstruction methods that dissolve the sense of self and separateness (Lindahl & Britton, 2019). To address the objective life conditions, the goal setting structure mapped Freund and Baltes’ (2002) elective selection. Here, participants chose a particular goal or set of goals to pursue as a means to improve their perception of quality of life (Teshale & Lachman, 2016).

Program Content

The program was divided into three sections. The first section included an introduction into the general findings of the quality of life and subjective well-being research. Participants were then introduced to the application of mindfulness in daily life and its relevance to well-being enhancement. Finally, they were introduced to ten different mindfulness attributes identified in the literature, before being trained in methods that would enhance these qualities. Mindfulness training methods broadly fell under categories similar to those defined by Lutz et al. (2008): attentional [focused attention (observation of the breath) and open monitoring (body scan)], constructive (loving kindness and compassion); and deconstructive (inquiry into thoughts and emotions). Participants were trained in class and were also given class exercises and set homework to practise (see Appendix A).

The ten attributes summarising some of the key qualities in mindfulness that were examined are: Present Moment Awareness (A present time, moment by moment awareness of all sensation); Neutrality (To neutrally observe thoughts, emotions or physical sensation as passing events); Clarity (The ability to identify clearly one’s beliefs and emotions); Regulation of Attention (The ability to both disengage from thought and direct attention where required); Thought Suspension (The ability to suspend engagement in thought and just be); Openness (The ability to have emotional openness to all experiences); Acceptance (An ability to accept/allow rather than avoid either good or bad feelings); Relaxation (A natural relaxation response to events); Response Freshness (To respond uniquely/freshly to each new event non-habitually); Equanimity (The capacity to treat others non-judgmentally/compassionately).

In Section two, participants were trained to apply the principles and methods of mindfulness to the five major life areas previously identified in the literature: relationships, work, money, health and leisure. For example, through education and exercises they explored how to apply the mindfulness attributes of non-judgmental awareness, interpersonal equanimity, or emotional openness to relationship conflict. Participants were then trained in goal setting theory and its relationship to quality of life and well-being. Participants rated their perception of quality of life with an expectation-outcome gap calculation, before being trained in setting SMART (specific, measurable, achievable, realistic and time-framed) goals and short term strategies across each of the five quality of life areas. In this section, participants were also set homework to follow through with their identified strategies and goals.

In section three, participants were trained in how to apply mindfulness to cultivate a robust mental state of well-being. As well-being typically is associated with areas such as self-concept (Hutz et al., 2014) and internal locus of control (Larson, 1989; Locke & Schattke, 2018; Strickland, 2016), participants received psycho-education to build a more robust sense of a “neutral, non-judgmental” self that was less prone to reacting to life events. They were also invited to the extension of this, as offered in traditional mindfulness (Kudesia & Nyima, 2014), which included the cultivation of the non-dual sense of self as “just witnessing”, pure awareness (Jones, 2019; Vago & Silbersweig, 2012). This state, in requiring less focus for self-regulation, has been found to increase attentional availability to phenomena (Raffone & Srinivasan, 2009), and is relevant to a more resourceful response to life conditions.

To complement psychoeducation, the third category of deconstructive methods (Lutz et al., 2008) was explored with participants practising self-inquiry (examination of the relationship between the self as observer and the objects it observes) and perspective taking methods, along with take-home exercises to consolidate gains and build stronger behavioural repertoires. For the 6-week group, each of the three sections was split into two sessions of 2 hours each (aggregating 12 hours of total content across the three sections plus homework) and for the 2-day group the three sections each constituted four hours of content plus homework.

Participants

As the identification of optimal recruitment methods require that recruitment and retention strategies need to be relevant to the target population and the research methodology used (Newington & Metcalfe, 2014), the rationale for the strategies selected included targeting a relevant demographic (psychologically healthy, English speaking adults), and reducing participant burden through easy access to portals and online surveys. The first recruitment strategy involved an online university student portal that provided information to students seeking to participate in research projects. Volunteers logged in and read general information about the program and an outline of time commitments. Because the portal was password protected, details of volunteers remained confidential.

The second recruitment method chosen was chain referral or snowball sampling. This sampling technique was selected as it is simple, cost-efficient, needs less planning and resources. These benefits were seen to outweigh any disadvantages such as less control over the sampling method and its representativeness. Questionnaires were handed out to people within several organisations, with each group providing a secure place for completed questionnaires to be returned in sealed envelopes.

Inclusion criteria for participation comprised a requirement that participants be a minimum of eighteen years old, and that they read the introductory brochure, university ethics pro forma letter, and complete the consent form, with the option to receive anonymised general results. As mindfulness meditation may be contraindicated under certain circumstances (Dobkin et al., 2011), exclusion criteria for participation included anyone whose primary language was other than English, or reported dependency on medical or psychiatric care, or anyone deemed to be “at risk” as determined by survey information and follow up interview. During the intervention, assessment of risk of harm with a possible consequence of exclusion included visible criteria (visible signs of self-harm or harm from others), or behavioural criteria such as disturbed affect (e.g. crying, anger).

Based on previous research (Sarenmalm et al., 2013), to detect differences between the three groups (6-week, 2-day, and control), 50 participants per group (150 participants in total) were needed to have adequate power (0.80) to detect medium effect sizes (d = 0.50). Due to possible attrition rates of approximately 50% (Dreher, 2009) a minimum of 300 participants in total were required.

To ensure that each participant had an equal chance of being allocated to any of the conditions, to eliminate any selection bias, and to test the efficacy of treatment (Suresh, 2011), a randomisation procedure was employed. A random number generator was used to assign participants to one of the two interventions or to the waitlist control condition. A list of random numbers between one and three was generated for each participant, based on the following specifications. Initially a maximum number of choices was entered (e.g. 350) followed by a minimum (1) and maximum value (3). A calculate button was then clicked to create a table of random numbers.

The primary researcher (who did not deliver the program to manage potential bias) was responsible for generating the random allocation sequence, the enrolment of participants, and assignation to condition. The randomisation sequence was concealed until interventions were assigned and participants were then informed of their allocated condition. As all three conditions were distinct (6-weekly, 2-day and waitlist control) it was not possible to blind participants or clinicians. In acknowledgement of this limitation, the treatment of the groups was standardised as much as possible (the course content for the 6-weekly and 2-day groups was the same), and objective, reliable assessment measures were used to reduce subjective bias and the need to blind data collectors or analysts (Karanicolas et al., 2010).

An initial sample of 342 subjects was randomly allocated into the three conditions (Weekly = 117, Two day = 105 and Control = 120). The age range was 18 – 80 years and a median age of 44 years with 79% females and 21% males. Because the initial online survey aimed to cast a wide net to recruit a large sample, a high attrition was expected. Hence, pre (baseline) scores from survey respondents who did not participate further were trimmed. Adjusting for attrition, 191 subjects (Weekly = 53, Two day = 49 and Control = 89) completed the program and filled out assessments across the three time periods. Complete data was collected from 167 participants and incomplete data was collected from the additional 24 participants (Weekly = 4, Two day = 8 and Control = 12).

Another 22 people started the program but either did not complete at least 4 sessions of the six weekly condition or only completed one day of the two day intervention. The completion criterion for the six week group was defined as 4-6 sessions, as participants who attended 1-3 sessions demonstrated a clear drop off trajectory toward the end of the program whilst most participants who missed 1 or 2 sessions still completed the final session. The completion criterion for the two day group was attendance of both days as 1 day attendance (equivalent to the first three sessions in the six week group) was not seen as sufficient content exposure.

Assessment

Five standardised instruments were chosen to assess the intervention (see Appendix B). Data was collected across the three time periods (pre, post and follow up) and analyses were done on summed change scores (Estrada et al., 2019) (pre-test score minus post-test score and post-test score minus follow up-test score). The Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) (Brown & Ryan, 2003) was chosen to assess mindfulness. The MAAS is a 15-item, self-report measure (e.g., “I find myself doing things without paying attention”) rated on a scale ranging from 1 to 6 which is totalled giving a range from 15–90. The scale correlates with other psychometric measures of mindfulness (Baer et al., 2006), and internal consistency reliability is high (Cronbach’s alpha = .82) (Brown & Ryan, 2003).

Quality of Life was measured with the Quality of Life Index (AQoL-(8-D) (Maxwell et al., 2016). The AQoL is a multi-attribute utility instrument to assess the psychosocial aspects of quality of life. Correlations between the AQoL and five other standardised multi-attribute utility instruments range from .49 to .65 and its Cronbach alpha coefficient is 0.96 (Richardson et al., 2013).

Subjective well-being was measured with the Personal Wellbeing Index–Adult (PWI-A) (International Wellbeing Group, 2006, 2013). In support of convergent validity, the PWI-A correlates .78 with Diener, Emmons, Larsen and Griffin’s (Diener et al., 1985) Satisfaction with life scale (Thomas, 2005). Cronbach’s alpha falls between .70 and .85 with a test-retest reliability correlation coefficient of 0.84 over an interval of 1-2 weeks (International Wellbeing Group, 2013; Lau et al., 2005).

As research has indicated a two-factor structure of general positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA) (Lac & Donaldson, 2018), and they represent the emotional components of SWB (Diener et al., 1985), the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) was included in the test battery (Watson et al., 1988). In the PANAS, respondents are asked to rate ten adjectives that depict positive and ten negative emotions using a five-point scale from “very slightly or not at all” to “extremely”. The 20-item self-report instrument has good internal consistency with a Cronbach’s alpha of .88 for PA, and .87 for NA and is robust across a wide range of populations (Dahiya & Rangnekar, 2019; Humboldt et al., 2017).

Similarly, as research has also identified both affective and cognitive components to subjective wellbeing, the Satisfaction with Life scale (SWL) (Diener et al., 1985) was chosen to assess change in participants’ satisfaction with their life as a whole. Whilst the scale does not assess satisfaction with life domains per se (e.g. health) it has been shown to identify changes in a single factor of life satisfaction (Whisman & Judd, 2016) including in response to clinical interventions (Pavot & Diener, 1993, 2009). The scale has five questions rated from (1) “Strongly disagree” to (7) “Strongly agree”. Diener et al. (1985) have reported a Cronbach’s alpha of .87 and test-retest reliability of .82 at two month retest.

A newly constructed measure, the Clinical Quality of Life Scale (CLINQOL), that matched the content of the MQW program was also chosen as it evaluated the subjective domains and objective domains of perceived well-being and quality of life in a single scale (see Appendix C). Made up of seven factors (Mental Style, Life Management, Relationships, Work, Money, Health, Leisure), items within each category formed internally consistent scales with Cronbach’s Alpha ranging from .504 to .904. The overall reliability of the scale was .907 which falls within the acceptable minimum of .70 for a newly developed scale (Lance et al., 2006; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). The CLINQOL was administered pre-post to participants of the two day program.

Procedure

The psycho-education and skills training content was delivered by a clinical psychologist registrar with previous experience in delivering the mindfulness-based cognitive therapy program (Segal et al., 2002). Treatment fidelity was maximized by supervised rehearsal of training content and post-delivery assessment. The three conditions were run within a 12 week period (the 6-week program ran from weeks 2-8 and the 2-day intensive program in week 8) with the online survey being simultaneously completed for the treatment and waitlist control conditions at the same pre, post and follow up time points (Pre = 0 weeks, Post = 8 weeks and Follow up = 12 weeks). Data was collected online or on location, at either the beginning or end of treatment as per time points.

Results

Prior to conducting the analyses, outliers and normality of the variables were examined, with the outlier labelling rule (Hoaglin et al., 1986) detecting no significant outliers. Most variables were normally distributed, except subjective well-being and negative affect. Logarithmic transformation of negative affect and square root transformation of subjective well-being adjusted the variables to within the normal range. To assess baseline differences in randomisation, a chi square test of independence/relatedness was conducted to investigate the relationship between discrete variables and the three conditions. This analysis was done post intervention, in accordance with the CONSORT statement that choosing covariates based on significance tests for baseline differences might lead to omissions of important covariates (Moher et al., 2010; De Boer et al., 2015). There was no significant difference between groups.

To assess if there may be some overlap in what is being measured across some variables, a correlation analysis of change scores (pre-test score minus post-test score), was initially conducted to investigate relationships among measures of mindfulness, quality of life, subjective well-being, positive affect, negative affect and life satisfaction. As there were significant relationships among dependent variables, MANOVA was conducted across three time points (pre, post and follow up) of scores at each time point to examine the effect of treatment upon the six dependent variables.

For univariate effects, planned contrasts (pre-post and post-follow up) were run to clarify interactions and, if significant, t-tests were conducted within each condition at pre, post and follow up and between conditions at each time point. Intention to treat (last score carried forward for missing values) analyses are presented for completers (analyses limited to participants with complete data provided no new information). Descriptive statistics, correlations between dependent variables, results from multivariate and univariate analyses, and t-tests are summarised in Tables 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 and 6.

Table 1 Intention to Treat mean scores at pre, post, and follow up for treatment (n=102) and control (n=89)
Table 2 Intention to treat pre-post and post-follow up change scores correlational matrix for treatment and control (n=191)
Table 3 Intention to Treat pre-post-follow up MANOVA outcomes (Wilk’s Lambda) for treatment (n=102) and control (n=89)
Table 4 Intention to Treat pre-post-follow up F ratios for treatment (n=102) and control (n=89)
Table 5 Intention to Treat mean scores at pre, post, and follow up for six week (n=53) and two day (n=49) participants
Table 6 Intention to Treat pre-post-follow F ratios for 6-week (n=53) and 2-day (n=49)

Treatment Versus Waitlist Control Intention to Treat Data

In the correlational analysis (Table 1) there were moderate correlations among most dependent variables (p**<.01) at pre-post, with the only exception being the relationship between mindfulness and life satisfaction indicating that, on the whole, the variables measured related elements. At post-follow up there were again correlations among most dependent variables other than mindfulness and negative affect (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Intention to treat treatment (n=102) and control (n=89) pre-post-follow up scores. For mindfulness (MAAS), quality of life (reverse scored) (AQoL), subjective well-being (PWI), positive and negative affect (PANAS) and life satisfaction (SWLS). Error bars represent standard errors. # indicates a pre-post or post-follow up difference and * indicates a difference between treatment and control (#p < 0.05, ##p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001)

As indicated in Table 2, at both pre-post and post-follow up, scores across all measures improved. This was reflected in the multivariate analyses with an effect between groups across time at pre-post but was not borne out at post-follow up (Table 3 and Table 4). Whilst only pre-post reached significance, post-follow up gains did not return to baseline but were maintained with marginal improvement. The weak to moderate significant interactions between groups across time (pre-post) for mindfulness, quality of life and subjective well-being, also suggests that each component of the three-part program was effective (Table 5).

Six Week Versus Two Day Intention to Treat Data

Multivariate analyses revealed an effect across time but no difference between groups and no interaction between group and time (Table 6). At follow up, however, the 2-day participants demonstrated a significant increase in self-reported mindfulness in comparison to their 6-week group counterparts, bringing them closer in line. This may suggest that mindfulness is more receptive to a graduated learning approach than other measured variables, and once participants had time to practise after their two day training, they were able to make similar gains to those of the 6-week group (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Intention to treat 6-week (n=53) and 2-day (n=49) pre-post-follow up scores. For mindfulness (MAAS), quality of life (reverse scored) (AQoL), subjective well-being (PWI), positive and negative affect (PANAS) and life satisfaction (SWLS). Error bars represent standard errors. # indicates a post-follow up difference and * indicates a difference between treatment and control (#p < 0.05, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01)

Two-Day Clinical Quality of Life Scale (CLINQOL) Data

A newly developed measure, the Clinical Quality of Life Scale (CLINQOL), that specifically matched the targeted quality of life domains of the MQW program, was used to evaluate the pre-post intervention changes in perceived quality of life. In view of the length of the multidimensional survey, the CLINQOL was administered only to participants pre and post of the two day condition with significant effects being reported (see Table 7).

Table 7 Raw scores (n=42) and intention to treat (n=59) pre and post mean scores and t-tests results of 2 day treatment group for quality of life (CLINQOL) (***p < 0.001)

Study 2

Method

To investigate what unique difference the full Mindfulness-based Quality of Life and Well-Being Program (MQW) might have in comparison to teaching just mindfulness alone, a follow up study was conducted comparing the full 6-week version of the MQW program to the mindfulness section of the program (the first 2-weeks of the full program with an additional 4 weeks of practise). To maintain program integrity, the same content was taught in both conditions; however, in the mindfulness alone section, the frequency and duration of times to practise the exercises were extended to expand the 2-week section to the same 6 week period.

Participants

The recruitment methods used in study 1 (online university student portals and snowball sampling techniques), participant selection criteria (a minimum of eighteen years old, not a reported dependency on medical or psychiatric care, provision of informed consent), and randomisation procedures, were also used in study 2.

Assessment

Four instruments were chosen to assess the intervention (see Appendix B) and data was again collected across the three time periods (pre, post and follow up), with analyses done on summed change scores (pre-test score minus post-test score and post-test score minus follow up-test score). The previously mentioned Clinical Quality of Life Scale (CLINQOL), Satisfaction with Life scale (SWLS) and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) were chosen to evaluate change in quality of life and subjective well-being, and to assess possible clinical changes in mental health, the abbreviated Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21) was also added. The DASS-21 correlates with other clinical measures of depression and anxiety (Coker et al., 2018) and internal consistency reliability amongst non-clinical adult samples is acceptable for all scales (Cronbach’s alpha is .91, .80, and .84 for Depression, Anxiety, and Stress respectively) (Sinclair et al., 2011).

Procedure

Treatment fidelity was managed with the content for both programs being delivered by university educated trainers who had previous experience in delivering the full MQW. The two conditions were run simultaneously within a 6 week period, and the evaluation surveys were completed for the both the MQW and the mindfulness program at the same pre, post and follow up time points (Pre = 0 weeks, Post = 6 weeks and Follow up = 10 weeks). Data was collected on location, at the beginning and end of treatment, and online for follow up.

An initial sample of 127 subjects registered online for the program and were randomly allocated into the two conditions (Mindfulness-based Quality of Life and Well-Being program and Mindfulness training only), and 106 subjects presented at session 1 to complete the pre-test. The age range was 18 – 77 years and a median age of 42.1 years with 88% females and 12% males. A total of 74 subjects (MQW = 45, Mindfulness = 29) began the program and filled out assessments across the three time periods. Of those who entered the program, 28 people did not complete at least 4 sessions of the six week program. As the completion criterion for sufficient program content exposure was defined as 4-6 sessions, these participant scores were trimmed, leaving a total of 46 participants (MQW = 26, Mindfulness = 20).

Results

Prior to conducting the analyses, a skewness analysis revealed all variables to be normally distributed with no significant outliers. A correlation analysis revealed significant relationships among dependent variables, so MANOVA was conducted across the pre, post and follow up time points. For univariate effects, planned contrasts (pre-post and post-follow up) were run to clarify interactions and, if significant, t-tests were conducted within each condition at pre, post and follow up and between conditions at each time point. Intention to treat (last score carried forward for missing values) analyses are presented for completers (analyses limited to participants with complete data again provided no new information). Descriptive statistics, correlations between dependent variables, results from multivariate and univariate analyses, and t-tests are summarised in Tables 8, 9 and 10.

Table 8 Intention to Treat mean scores at pre, post, and follow up for full MQW Program (n=26) and Mindfulness Section only (n=20) participants
Table 9 Intention to treat pre-post and post-follow up change scores correlational matrix for MQW and mindfulness only (n=46)
Table 10 Intention to Treat pre-post-follow F ratios for full MQW Program (n=26) and Mindfulness Section only (n=20) participants

MQW Versus Mindfulness Only Intention to Treat Data

Multivariate analyses revealed an effect across time but no effect between groups or an interaction between group and time (Table 10). Univariate analyses revealed that the mindfulness only participants demonstrated improvements in primarily the clinical indicators of negative affect, anxiety and overall mental health (DASS-21) along with quality of life (CLINQOL), compared to their MQW counterparts. This may indicate that extra time allocated for mindfulness exercises over psycho-education produces greater change (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Intention to treat full MQW Program (n=26) and Mindfulness Section only (n=20) participants. For quality of life (CLINQOL), subjective well-being (SWLS), positive and negative affect (PANAS) and mental health (DASS-21, DASS-D and DASS-A). Error bars represent standard errors and # indicates a pre-post or pre-post-fw difference (#p < 0.05)

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to develop and test, with a random sample from the general population, a new quality of life and well-being enhancement program. By examining the effect upon six dependent variables that, based on prior research and theory, were predicted to be modified by such interventions, the research sought to explore the efficacy of the newly developed Mindfulness-based Quality of Life and Well-being Program (MQW).

Presented in both a six week graduated format and two day intensive format, the main hypothesis was that the program would improve scores in comparison to waitlist control, across the six dependent variables of mindfulness, quality of life, subjective well-being, positive and negative affect and life satisfaction. This hypothesis was supported with analysis of the data revealing that there were significant differences for treatment versus control in five of the six variables (except life satisfaction) at post-test. Differences between groups were maintained at follow up for most variables and strengthened for mindfulness. These findings were further strengthened by significant pre-post gains as measured by the new Clinical Quality of Life Scale (CLINQOL).

The intervention content was presented in three sections: mindfulness training; psycho-education and goal setting in life areas associated with quality of life; and psycho-education and exercises to improve well-being. The aim was to train participants in these three areas and to determine whether participants reported any changes. Significant findings specifically in the mindfulness, quality of life and subjective well-being measures give some indication of program efficacy and suggest that the program achieved its aims.

With respect to each specific component of the program, in the mindfulness training section participants were introduced to ten mindfulness attributes and trained in exercises to improve these characteristics. Progress was borne out at both post treatment and follow up against control, suggesting that this method is an effective means to improve self-report of mindfulness as reported using Brown and Ryan’s (2003) mindfulness scale.

In the quality of life section, participants were taught how to improve five quality of life domains (relationships, work, money, health and leisure) (Diener et al., 1999; Tay et al., 2014), and then were taught to write and execute short and long term goals using time-framed strategies to assist goal achievement. One could speculate that significant increases in the Quality of Life Index scores (Richardson et al., 2013) offer further evidence that goal setting has some bearing on increased perception of quality of life (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Locke & Latham, 2013).

Finally, in the well-being section participants received psycho-education on the origins of self-esteem and well-being and completed exercises to build well-being. Participants aimed to achieve this independent of the status of the objective life domains in section two that were previously worked on.

The reported increases in the Personal Wellbeing Index (PWI-A, International Wellbeing Group, 2006) and in the Positive Affect and Negative Affect measures (PANAS) (Watson et al., 1988) indicate successful uptake of the program. Of interest, however, is why the well-researched Satisfaction with Life scale (Diener et al., 1985; Pavot & Diener, 2009) did not show signs of improvement. One possibility is that life satisfaction captures only one dimension of subjective well-being - the cognitive evaluation of an individual’s satisfaction with their life. As this scale describes an enduring trait it may not be amenable to change after a six week or two day intervention, although life satisfaction is thought to be sensitive to clinical interventions (Pavot & Diener, 1993, 2009). By contrast, significant findings in the more multidimensional Quality of Life Index (Richardson et al., 2013) and Personal Wellbeing Index (International Wellbeing Group, 2006) lend support to the inclusion of measures that capture a broader picture of each construct.

As the program was made up of discrete sections that focused on different topics, it is difficult to identify which components had most impact. For example, would mindfulness training alone (Hölzel et al., 2011) be sufficient to create the reported gains in quality of life and well-being, or was the addition of the quality of life goal setting and well-being training pivotal in that result? Brown et al. (2009) reasoned that mindfulness could reduce the “achievement-aspiration gap” (the bigger the gap between expectations and outcomes, the lower the evaluation of one’s quality of life) by assisting someone to “want what one has,” which in turn leads to an increase in the perception of quality of life. Hence, mindfulness training may have been key to the success of this program.

In support of the notion that mindfulness training can improve existing happiness programs, Smith, Compton and West (Smith et al., 1995) found that the addition of meditation to Fordyce’s (1983, 1988) happiness enhancement program resulted in a further improvement in dependent measures in comparison to the original group. However, as mindfulness now has a strong presence in the intervention literature, a richer exploration of the constructs and processes that are proposed to be responsible for well-being outcomes could have benefit for future intervention design.

For example, as the third section of the program focused on the role in well-being of self-concept (Hutz et al., 2014), positive self-esteem (Robins et al., 2001), and internal locus of control (Strickland, 2016), a more in-depth inquiry into how the concept of the self impacts the perception of quality of life could prove useful for future programs. As such, it could build on the extension suggested in traditional mindfulness (Kudesia & Nyima, 2014) to cultivate the non-dual, non-self referencing experience (Natarajan, 2000; Vago & Silbersweig, 2012). In view of its reduced focus on self-regulation (Lutz et al., 2008; Raffone & Srinivasan, 2009), this may be associated with more robust and enduring experiences of well-being and generate a more resourceful response to life conditions. Such an exploration could feed back to the quality of life and well-being research and impact the development of interventions.

In terms of different intervention delivery formats, previous research suggesting that there is little difference between compressed and progressive learning formats (Arrey, 2005; Wlodkowski & Westover, 1999) was again borne out in this study. The two-day group improved more from post-treatment to follow-up in mindfulness than the 6-week group; however, this increase brought them in line with the 6-week group at follow-up. This seems reasonable as at post-test the two-day group had only just been taught the ten mindfulness skills, whilst the six-week group had already practised these skills over the previous six weeks leading up to post-test.

The two-day group’s improvement over the following four weeks, in comparison to the six-week group who had already achieved most of their gains, does, however, support the evidence that people benefit from time to practice mindfulness (Cahn et al., 2013; Cullen, 2011). Whilst these outcomes suggest that current research on program design appears to generalise to a well-being enhancement setting, in view of resource allocation (participant time and training budgets), this area deserves further investigation.

Non-completers who provided data at post-treatment and/or follow-up (22 participants) received no beneficial effect from completing either one to three sessions of the six-week program or only one day of the two-day program. This finding is of interest and seems to be in line with research that people need to practise meditation on a regular basis to receive any sustained benefit (Delmonte, 1985; Jones, 2018; Zettle & Gird, 2016). However, there may be a motivational factor involved in this outcome, in that most of the six-week completer cohort (minimum attendance was four out of a possible six sessions) who missed one or two sessions still completed the final session, suggesting some level of commitment to completing the process. In contrast, no participant who attended three or fewer classes attended the final session, suggesting a possible lack of engagement or motivation, a variable found to be predictive of success (Locke & Schattke, 2018).

As the primary interest in the analysis of the program was its efficacy, a waitlist control condition was chosen as it provided an untreated comparison for the active experimental group. However, a limitation of choosing a waitlist control over an established program serving as an active comparator is that is remains unclear if the newly developed program is superior compared to other programs. As such, in future studies, the Mindfulness-based Quality of Life and Well-being Program (MQW) should be compared against administration of programs such as the Mindfulness-based Stress Reduction program (MBSR).

In support of Van Dam et al.’s (Van Merriënboer & Kirschner, 2017) critique that only 1% of research has been done outside of research contexts, the follow up community study, that compared the full MQW program to an expanded version of the mindfulness section, gave further information on the influential role that exercises may play in content uptake. As the mindfulness only section with its increased time to practice proved more effective than the full program in decreasing negative affect and other mental health measures, further iterations of the full program could do well to increase time allocated to practise the skillsets being taught in the psycho-education portions of the program.

In terms of evaluation of adverse effects (Compson, 2018), there may have been an optimism bias in the evaluation of the program, in that it sought to determine its efficacy alone rather than its potential harm. This was partially addressed in a brief program evaluation-questionnaire that included the question “Overall what were the least useful features of the program for you?”. However as there are no reliable estimates of harm due to mindfulness-based interventions (Baer et al., 2019), it would have been of value to identify any adverse effects due to exposure to the trial of a new program. For example, participants could have been evaluated for increased symptoms after treatment (Hirshberg et al., 2020) using the Global Severity Index (GSI), which examines psychological symptom severity (Derogatis, 2012). Furthermore, participants who dropped out during the program could have been sent an evaluation questionnaire that sought to address these questions.

In respect of the evaluation process itself, intervention efficacy rested on standardised self-report scales and the newly developed Clinical Quality of Life Scale (CLINQOL). Whilst self-report is the most common and easiest modality to assess intervention effects, other forms of measurement could have provided richer data to reduce false optimism, social desirability biases or the halo effect (Deaton, 2018). For example, the collection of behavioural data (Levinson et al., 2014) such as frequency and duration of practice during training could assist a richer evaluation of effects amongst participants. Similarly, inclusion of more eudaimonic scales such as Peterson et al.’s (2005) Orientations to Happiness Questionnaire, or the Scales of Psychological Well-Being (Ryff, 1989) which includes self-acceptance, purpose in life, personal growth and development dimensions, could also have yielded interesting data.

As the literature on subjective wellbeing and mindfulness has been criticised for insufficient construct and criterion validation of measures (Lepper, 1998; Van Dam et al., 2010), it could have also been of value to include observer or other reports (Ashton, 2013). For example, a brief scale could have been added to the overall survey to be completed by both participant and a friend or family member at pre, post and follow-up. In addition, data on months/years of meditation or mindfulness practice (Hauswald et al., 2015; van Leeuwen et al., 2009) at pre-test could also be gathered and correlated with scores on Brown and Ryan’s (2003) Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale.

Whilst acknowledging the phenomenon of attrition in any program delivery, there could be some provision for follow-up after missed sessions to preserve participation. It is possible that a non-coercive follow-up reminder to participants who missed sessions may encourage increased participation in the program which, in turn, would increase sample size and power in the analysis. Finally, a way of adding to the richness of the intervention, along with homework given at the end of each training section, could be to collect, evaluate and return homework as an opportunity to give more personalised feedback to participants.

It is of relevance, in line with a current critique within the mindfulness research, to note that the current MQW intervention falls more into a second-generation mindfulness-based intervention (SG-MBI) than a first generation mindfulness-based intervention (FG-MBI) as described by Van Gordon, Shonin and Griffiths (Van Gordon et al., 2015; Van Gordon & Shonin, 2020). Key differences relevant to the MQW and its focus on quality of life is that, in line with SG-MBIs, practitioners employ not only ‘non-judge-mental’ awareness (a key FG-MBI mindfulness descriptor), but also encourage an active participation in the present moment and to respond in an adaptive manner.

As SG-MBIs also teach mindfulness along with other meditative practices and principles (impermanence of phenomena, emptiness/non-self, and loving-kindness meditations) (Shonin et al., 2014), the current intervention, with its suite of mindfulness and well-being methods, aimed to assist in the development of this next exciting stage of mindfulness-based interventions.

Overall, these findings suggest that the hybrid program (mindfulness, goal setting and psycho-education) was effective in improving mindfulness, quality of life, subjective well-being and positive and negative affect. Such results provide preliminary evidence that a multi-dimensional training approach that teaches mindfulness, goal setting and well-being enhancement, and that includes both psycho-education and skills training can be a valuable and effective intervention model. This broader type of curriculum may be able to offer participants the opportunity to not only learn mindfulness and goal setting skills but in doing so, learn both the theory to develop well-being and quality of life and the exercises to assist in its achievement and maintenance.

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.