Skip to main content
Log in

Good people do bad things: How anxiety promotes unethical behavior through intuitive and automatic processing

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although researchers have found that emotions affect unethical behavior, few studies have uncovered the mechanism between anxiety and unethical behavior. The present study investigated how anxiety might influence unethical behavior based on the dual-process theory. In Experiment 1, a recall writing task was used to induce anxiety, and unethical behavior was assessed during an Interaction Task. Results showed that compared with participants who felt neutral emotions, those who felt anxiety were more likely to act unethically. In Experiment 2, we induced emotions by having participants watch videos and measured unethical behavior through a visual perception task. The results replicated findings in Experiment 1 and found that intuitive and automatic processing mediated the relationship between anxiety and unethical behavior. Collectively, our work reveals that anxiety might increase unethical behavior because individuals who feel anxious are more likely to engage in intuitive automatic processing that shifts attention from moral standards to self-interest, and thus to behave unethically. Both theoretical and practical implications of the results are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ackerman, R., & Zalmanov, H. (2012). The persistence of the fluency–confidence association in problem solving. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 1187–1192.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychological Association. (2012). Workplace survey: Psychologically healthy workplace program. Retrieved from https://www.apa.org/news/press/releases/phwa/workplace-survey.Pdf.

  • Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bensi, L., & Giusberti, F. (2007). Trait anxiety and reasoning under uncertainty. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 827–838.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bieleke, M., Gollwitzer, P. M., Oettingen, G., & Fischbacher, U. (2017). Social value orientation moderates the effects of intuition versus reflection on responses to unfair ultimatum offers. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30, 569–581.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, N. G., Lewin, J. E., & Sager, J. K. (2009). A model of stress and coping and their influence on individual and organizational outcomes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 75, 197–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, A. W., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2011). Can nervous nelly negotiate? How anxiety causes negotiators to make low first offers, exit early, and earn less profit. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115, 43–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, M., Lewton, M., & Ashwin, C. (2016). Reasoning on the autism spectrum: A dual process theory account. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2115–2125.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, M. E., & Treviño, L. K. (2006). Socialized charismatic leadership, values congruence, and deviance in work groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 954–962.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagley, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 212–252). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cushman, F., Young, L., & Hauser, M. (2006). The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgment: Testing three principles of harm. Psychological Science, 17, 1082–1089.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darlow, A. L., & Sloman, S. A. (2010). Two systems of reasoning: Architecture and relation to emotion. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 382–393.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Effelsberg, D., Solga, M., & Gurt, J. (2014). Transformational leadership and follower’s unethical behavior for the benefit of the company: A two-study investigation. Journal of Business Ethics, 120, 81–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, G. M. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition & Emotion, 6, 409–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fan, W., Zhon, Y. P., Li, H. Y., Meng, C. Y., You, C., & Fu, X. L. (2016). The influence of self-control in the perceived of deception and deception. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 42, 317–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19, 25–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaspar, J. P., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2013). The emotion deception model: A review of deception in negotiation and the role of emotion in deception. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 6, 160–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F., & Margolis, J. D. (2011). Bringing ethics into focus: How regulatory focus and risk preferences influence (un)ethical behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115, 145–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F., Norton, M., & Ariely, D. (2010). The counterfeit self: The deceptive costs of faking it. Psychological Science, 21, 712–720.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F., Brooks, A. W., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2012). Anxiety, advice, and the ability to discern: Feeling anxious motivates individuals to seek and use advice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 497–512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gneezy, U. (2005). Deception: The role of consequences. The American Economic Review, 95, 384–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goel, N., Rao, H., Durmer, J. S., & Dinges, D. F. (2009). Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Seminars in Neurology, 29, 320–339.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Grecucci, A., Giorgetta, C., Brambilla, P., Zuanon, S., Perini, L., Balestrieri, M., et al. (2013). Anxious ultimatums: How anxiety disorders affect socioeconomic behavior. Cognition & Emotion, 27, 230–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halali, E., Bereby-Meyer, Y., & Ockenfels, A. (2013). Is it all about the self? The effect of self-control depletion on ultimatum game proposers. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 240–248.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Halali, E., Berebymeyer, Y., & Meiran, N. (2014). Between self-interest and reciprocity: The social bright side of self-control failure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 745–754.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, E. J., Van Marle, H. J. F., Ossewaarde, L., Henckens, M. J. A. G., Qin, S., Van Kestern, M. T. R., & Fernández, G. (2011). Stress-related noradrenergic activity prompts large-scale neural network reconfiguration. Science, 25, 1151–1153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kishgephart, J. J., Harrison, D. A., & Treviño, L. K. (2010). Bad apples, bad cases, and bad barrels: Meta-analytic evidence about sources of unethical decisions at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knoch, D., Pascual-Leone, A., Meyer, K., Treyer, V., & Fehr, E. (2006). Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science, 314, 829–832.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knoch, D., Gianotti, L. R. R., Baumgartner, T., & Fehr, E. (2010). A neural marker of costly punishment behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 337–342.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kouchaki, M., & Desai, S. D. (2015). Anxious, threatened, and also unethical: How anxiety makes individuals feel threatened and commit unethical acts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 360–375.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. M., Kim, Y. A., Park, I. J., & Sohn, Y. W. (2017). The effects of anxiety on attention problems and rule-breaking behavior: The moderating effect of work adjustment in the workplace. Current Psychology, 2, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, E. E., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2014). Are liars ethical? On the tension between benevolence and honesty. Social Science Electronic Publishing, 53, 107–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Licht, J. G. J., Leroy, S., & Vohs, K. (2013). Unethical behavior for self or collective benefit: The role of self- construal. Academy of Management Annual Meeting Proceedings, 2013, 17391–17391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259–289.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masicampo, E. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2008). Toward a physiology of dual-process reasoning and judgment: Lemonade, willpower, and expensive rule-based analysis. Psychological Science, 19, 255–260.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional intelligence (pp. 3–31). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J. M., Trougakos, J. P., & Cheng, B. H. (2016). Are anxious workers less productive workers? It depends on the quality of social exchange. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101, 279–292.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. E., & Cohen, S. (2001). Psychological interventions and the immune system: A meta-analytic review and critique. Health Psychology Official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology American Psychological Association, 20, 47–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moran, S., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2008). When better is worse: Envy and the use of deception. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 1, 3–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Motro, D., Ordóñez, L. D., Pittarello, A., & Welsh, D. T. (2016). Investigating the effects of anger and guilt on unethical behavior: A dual-process approach. Journal of Business Ethics, 2016, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ploeger, N. A., & Bisel, R. S. (2013). The role of identification in giving sense to unethical organizational behavior: Defending the organization. Management Communication Quarterly, 27, 155–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717–731.

    Google Scholar 

  • Primi, C., Morsanyi, K., Chiesi, F., Donati, M. A., & Hamilton, J. (2015). The development and testing of a new version of the cognitive reflection test applying item response theory (IRT). Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29, 453–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seah, S. L., & Ang, R. P. (2008). Differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in asian adolescents: Relations to narcissism, anxiety, schizotypal traits, and peer relations. Aggressive Behavior, 34, 553–562.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shalvi, S., Eldar, O., & Berebymeyer, Y. (2012). Honesty requires time (and lack of justifications). Psychological Science, 23, 1264–1270.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., Schwarz, N., & Gschneidinger, E. (1985). Happiness and reminiscing: The role of time perspective, affect, and mode of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1460–1469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sütterlin, S., Herbert, C., Schmitt, M., Kübler, A., & Vögele, C. (2011). Overcoming selfishness: Reciprocity, inhibition, and cardiac-autonomic control in the ultimatum game. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 173–181.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. (2002). Shame and guilt. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomson, K. S., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2016). Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test. Judgment and Decision making, 11, 99–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2011). The cognitive reflection test as a predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks. Memory & Cognition, 39, 1275–1289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vohs, K. D. (2006). Self-regulatory resources power the reflective system: Evidence from five domains. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16, 217–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, D. T., & Ordóñez, L. D. (2014). The dark side of consecutive high performance goals: Linking goal setting, depletion, and unethical behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 123, 79–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, G. (1999). Anxiety and decision making with delayed resolution of uncertainty. Theory and Decision, 46, 159–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yip, J. A., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2016). Mad and misleading: Incidental anger promotes deception. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 137, 207–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, M. (2016). Neural basis of trait anxiety and neural mechanism of trait anxiety individuals under uncertainty. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Southwest University.

  • Zhang, Y. M., Du, X. F., & Wang, X. X. (2015). Influence of anxiety and the degree of benevolence on advice-taking. Journal of Psychological Science, 2015(38), 1155–1161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhong, C. B. (2011). The ethical dangers of deliberative decision making. Administrative Science Quarterly, 56, 1–25.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The present research was supported by The Project of the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 31200795), Project of the National Social Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 16CGL044) and self-determined research funds of CCNU from the colleges’ basic research and operation of MOE (Grant No. CCNU14Z02015).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hongyu Ma.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of Interest

The Author’s declares that they has no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhang, H., Shi, Y., Zhou, Z.E. et al. Good people do bad things: How anxiety promotes unethical behavior through intuitive and automatic processing. Curr Psychol 39, 720–728 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9789-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9789-7

Keywords

Navigation