Skip to main content
Log in

Dynamic visual perception and reading development in Chinese school children

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The development of reading skills may depend to a certain extent on the development of basic visual perception. The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia assumes that deficits in the magnocellular pathway, indicated by less sensitivity in perceiving dynamic sensory stimuli, are responsible for a proportion of reading difficulties experienced by dyslexics. Using a task that measures coherent motion detection threshold, this study examined the relationship between dynamic visual perception and reading development in Chinese children. Experiment 1 compared the performance of 27 dyslexics and their age- and IQ-matched controls in the coherent motion detection task and in a static pattern perception task. Results showed that only in the former task did the dyslexics have a significantly higher threshold than the controls, suggesting that Chinese dyslexics, like some of their Western counterparts, may have deficits in magnocellular pathway. Experiment 2 examined whether dynamic visual processing affects specific cognitive processes in reading. One hundred fifth-grade children were tested on visual perception and reading-related tasks. Regression analyses found that the motion detection threshold accounted for 11% and 12%, respectively, variance in the speed of orthographic similarity judgment and in the accuracy of picture naming after IQ and vocabulary size were controlled. The static pattern detection threshold could not account for any variance. It is concluded that reading development in Chinese depends to a certain extent on the development of dynamic visual perception and its underlying neural pathway and that the impact of visual development can be specifically related to orthographic processing in reading Chinese.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ben-Shachar, M., Dougherty, R. F., Deutsch, G. K., & Wandell, B. A. (2007). Contrast responsivity in MT+ correlates with phonological awareness and reading measures in children. Neuroimage, 37, 1396–1406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boden, C., & Giaschi, D. (2007). M-stream deficits and reading-related visual processes in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 346–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boets, B., Wouters, J., van Wieringen, A., et al. (2006). Auditory temporal information processing in preschool children at family risk for dyslexia: Relations with phonological abilities and developing literacy skills. Brain and Language, 97, 64–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borsting, E., Ridder, W. H., III, Dideck, K., Kelley, C., Matsui, L., & Motoyama, J. (1996). The presence of a magnocellular deficit depends on the type of dyslexia. Vision Research, 36, 1047–1053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosse, M. L., Tainturier, M. J., & Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104, 198–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L. L., & Brayant, P. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organization as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature, 271, 746–747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L. L., & Brayant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connexion. Nature, 301, 419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L. L., & Brayant, P. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics’ phonological deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874–886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casco, C., & Prunetti, E. (1996). Visual search of good and poor readers: effects with targets having single and combined features. Perceptual Motor Skills, 82, 1155–1167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, K., Fujita, H., Kanno, I., Miura, S., & Tanaka, K. (1995). Human cortical regions activated by wide-field visual motion: an H2(15)O PET study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, 413–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, K. K. H., McBride-Chang, C., Wong, S. W. L., Cheung, H., Penney, T. B., & Ho, C. (2008). The role of visual and auditory temporal processing for Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 58, 15–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conlon, E., Sanders, M., & Zapart, S. (2004). Temporal processing in poor adult readers. Neuropsychologia, 42, 142–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conlon, E. G., Sanders, M. A., & Wright, C. M. (2009). Relationships between global motion and global form processing, practice, cognitive and visual processing in adults with dyslexia or visual discomfort. Neuropsychologia, 47, 907–915.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornelissen, P., & Hansen, P. (1998). Motion detection, letter position encoding, and single word reading. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 155–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornelissen, P., Hansen, P., Hutton, J., Evangelinou, V., & Stein, J. F. (1998). Magnocellular visual function and children’s single word reading. Vision Research, 38, 471–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demb, J. B., Boynton, G. M., Best, M., & Heeger, D. (1998). Psychophysical evidence for a magnocellular deficit in dyslexia. Vision research, 38, 1555–1559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eden, G. F., Van Meter, J. W., Rumsey, J. M., & Zeffiro, T. A. (1996). The visual deficit theory of developmental dyslexia. Neuroimage, 4, S108–S117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eden, G. F., Van Meter, J. W., Rumsey, J. M., Maisog, J., Woods, R., & Zeffiro, T. A. (1996). Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging. Nature, 382, 66–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, V. T., Giaschi, D. E., Dougherty, R. F., Edgell, D., Bjornson, B. H., Lyons, C., et al. (2004). Psychophysical indexes of temporal processing abnormalities in children with developmental dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25, 321–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Facoetti, A., Paganoni, P., Turatto, M., Marzola, V., & Mascetti, G. G. (2000). Visual–spatial attention in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 36, 109–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Ruffino, M., et al. (2010). Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1011–1025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, B., & Hartnegg, K. (2000). Effects of visual training on saccade control in dyslexia. Perception, 29, 531–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, I. K., & Forster, C. J. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 116–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1981). Experimental approaches to developmental dyslexia: An introduction. Psychological Research, 43, 97–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 20–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, P. C. (2001). Global form and global motion processing programs. London: Department of Physiology, Oxford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, P. C., Stein, J. F., Orde, S. R., Winter, J. L., & Talcott, J. B. (2001). Are dyslexics’ visual deficits limited to measures of dorsal stream function? NeuroReport, 12, 1527–1530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hari, R., Valta, M., & Uutela, K. (1999). Prolonged attentional dwell time in dyslexic adults. Neuroscience Letters, 271, 202–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. (1997). The importance of phonological awareness and verbal short-term memory to children’s success in learning to read Chinese. Psychologia, 40, 211–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C., & Bryant, P. (1997). Phonological skills are important in learning to read Chinese. Developmental Psychology, 33, 946–951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C., Chan, D., Lee, S., Tsang, S., & Luan, V. (2004). Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 91, 43–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C., Chan, D., Tsang, S., & Lee, S. (2000). The Hong Kong test of specific learning-difficulties in reading and writing (HKT-SpLD), August (Researchth ed.). Hong Kong: Hong Kong Specific Learning Difficulties Research Team.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hood, M., & Conlon, E. (2004). Visual and auditory temporal processing and early reading development. Dyslexia, 10, 234–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, C. F., & Catts, H. W. (1998). The role of phonological processing in early reading ability: what we can learn from Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 55–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, H. S., & Hanley, J. R. (1994). Phonological awareness and visual skills in learning to read Chinese and English. Cognition, 54, 73–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, H. S., & Hanley, J. R. (1995). Phonological awareness and visual skills in learning to read Chinese and English. Cognition, 54, 73–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, H. S., & Hanley, J. R. (1997). A longitudinal study of phonological awareness, visual skills and Chinese reading acquisition among first graders in Taiwan. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20(2), 249–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulslander, J., Talcott, J., Witton, C., DeFries, J., Pennington, B., Wadsworth, S., et al. (2004). Sensory processing, reading, IQ, and attention. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 274–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, A., Bruno, A., Watanabe, J., Quansah, B., Patel, N., Dakin, S., et al. (2008). Visually-based temporal distortion in dyslexia. Vision Research, 48, 1852–1858.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kevan, A., & Pammer, K. (2008). Visual processing deficits in preliterate children at familial risk for dyslexia. Vision Research, 48, 2835–2839.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kevan, A., & Pammer, K. (2009). Predicting early reading skills from pre-reading measures of dorsal stream functioning. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3174–3181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laycock, R., & Crewther, S. G. (2008). Towards an understanding of the role of the magnocellular advantage in fluent reading. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(8), 1494–1506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leck, K. J., Weekes, B. S., & Chen, M. J. (1995). Visual and phonological pathways to the lexicon: evidence from Chinese readers. Memory & Cognition, 23, 468–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, T., Walsh, V., & Lavidor, M. (2010). Dorsal stream modulation of visual word recognition in skilled readers. Vision Research, 50, 883–888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovegrove, W. J., Bowling, A., Badcock, D., & Blackwood, M. (1980). Specific reading disability: Differences in contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency. Science, 210, 439–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Petersen, O. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Custodio, R., & Szeszulski, P. A. (1993). Development of phonological and orthographic skill: a two-year longitudinal study of dyslexic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 64–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (2004). Children’s literacy development. London: Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Chang, L. (1995). Memory, print exposure, and metacognition: components of reading in Chinese children. International Journal of Psychology, 30(5), 607–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. (2000). Developmental issues in Chinese children’s character acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 50–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meng, X. (2000). The lexical representation and processing in Chinese-speaking developmental dyslexia. Doctoral dissertation: Beijing Normal University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meng, X., Zhou, X., Zeng, B., Kong, R., & Zhuang, J. (2002). Visual perceptual skills and reading abilities in Chinese-speaking children (in Chinese). Acta Psychologica Sinica, 34(1), 16–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meng, X., Tian, X., Jian, J., & Zhou, X. (2007). Orthographic and phonological processing in Chinese dyslexic children: An ERP study on sentence reading. Brain Research, 1179, 119–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meng, X., Jian, J., Shu, H., Tian, X., & Zhou, X. (2008). ERP correlates of the development of orthographical and phonological processing during Chinese sentence reading. Brain Research, 1219, 91–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E., & Gibson, L. Y. (2008). Investigating the functional integrity of the dorsal visual pathway in autism and dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2593–2596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F. (2004). Neurobiology of dyslexia? A reinterpretation of the data. Trends in Neurosciences, 27, 720–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Catelotte, J. M., White, S., et al. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841–865.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reid, A. A., Szczerbinski, M., Iskierka-Kasperek, E., & Hansen, P. (2007). Cognitive profiles of adult developmental dyslexics: Theoretical implications. Dyslexia, 13, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridder, W. H., Borsting, E., & Banton, T. (2001). All developmental dyslexia subtypes display an elevated motion coherence threshold. Optometry and Vision Science, 78, 510–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Cheng, Y. S., & Zhang, H. C. (1989). The naming consistency, familiarity, representation consistency and visual complexity of 235 pictures (in Chinese). Acta Psychologica Sinica, 21(4), 389–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, X. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: implications for learning to read. Child Development, 74(1), 27–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siok, W. T., & Fletcher, P. (2001). The role of phonological awareness and visual–orthographic skills in Chinese reading acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 37(6), 886–899.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slaghuis, W. L., & Lovegrove, W. J. (1985). Spatial-frequency-dependent visible persistence and specific reading disability. Brain and Cognition, 4, 219–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slaghuis, W. L., & Ryan, J. F. (1999). Spatio-temporal contrast sensitivity, coherent motion, and visible persistence in developmental dyslexia. Vision Research, 39, 651–668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Song, H., Zhang, H., & Shu, H. (1995). The role of phonology and orthography and its development in Chinese reading. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 27, 139–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L. S., Gottardo, A., Chiappe, P., & Sidhu, R. (1997). Subtypes of developmental dyslexia: differences in phonological and orthographic coding. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia (pp. 115–142). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, J. F. (1994). A visual defect in dyslexics? In A. Fawcett & R. Nicolson (Eds.), Dyslexia in children: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 137–156). Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, J. F., & Talcott, J. (1999). Impaired neuronal timing in developmental dyslexia—The magnocellular hypothesis. Dyslexia, 5, 59–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, J. F., & Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read: the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trend in Neurological Science, 20, 147–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, J. F., Richardson, A. J., & Fowler, M. S. (2000). Monocular occlusion can improve binocular control and reading in dyslexics. Brain, 123, 164–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, H. W., Stigler, J. W., Lucker, G. W., Hsu, C. C., & Kitamura, S. (1982). Reading disabilities: the case of Chinese, Japanese and English. Child Development, 53, 1164–1181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Talcott, J. B., Hansen, P. C., Willis-Owen, C., McKinnell, I. W., Richardson, A. J., & Stein, J. F. (1998). Visual magnocellular impairment in adult developmental dyslexics. Neuro-Opthalmology, 20, 187–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Talcott, T. B., Witton, C., McLean, M. F., Hansen, P. C., Rees, A., Green, G. G. R., et al. (2000). Dynamic sensory sensitivity and children’s word decoding skills. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 2952–2957.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tzeng, O. J. L., & Wang, W. S. Y. (1983). The first two R’s. Scientific American, 71, 238–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaina, L. M., Lemay, M., Bienfang, D. C., Choi, A. Y., & Nakayama, K. (1990). Intact “biological motion” and “structure from motion” perception in a patient with impaired motion mechanisms: A case study. Visual Neuroscience, 5, 353–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vidyasagar, T. R., & Pammer, K. (1999). Impaired visual search in dyslexia relates to the role of the magnocellular pathway in attention. Neuroreport, 10, 1283–1287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vidyassagar, T. R., & Pammer, K. (2009). Dyslexia: A deficit in visual–spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 57–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, X. L., & Tao, B. P. (1996). The scale and assessment of vocabulary for primary school (in Chinese). Shanghai: Shanghai Educational.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, S., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Hansen, P., Swettenham, J., Frith, U., et al. (2006). The role of sensorimotor impairments in dyslexia: A multiple case study of dyslexic children. Developmental Science, 9, 237–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, M. J., Stuart, G. W., Castles, A., & McAnally, K. I. (2003). Contrast sensitivity in subgroups of developmental dyslexia. Vision Research, 43, 467–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Witton, C., Talcott, J. B., Hansen, P. C., Richardson, A. J., Griffiths, T. D., Rees, A., et al. (1998). Sensitivity to dynamic auditory and visual stimuli predicts nonword reading ability in both dyslexic and normal readers. Current Biology, 8, 791–797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeki, S., Watson, J. D. G., Lueck, C. J., Friston, K. J., Kennard, C., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1991). A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 641–649.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, H. C., & Wang, X. P. (1985). Raven standard progressive matrices: Chinese city revision. Beijing: The National Revision Collaborative Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, C. F., Zhang, J. H., Chang, S., Zhou, J., & Yin, R. (1998). A study of cognitive profiles of Chinese learners’ reading disabilities. Acta Psychologica Sinica (in Chinese), 30, 50–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, Y. G. (1978). To what degree are the ‘phonetics’ of present-day Chinese characters still phonetic? (in Chinese) Zhongguo Yuwen, 146, 172–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1999). Phonology, orthography, and lexical semantic activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 579–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2000). The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1245–1265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

This study was supported by grants from the Natural Science Foundation of China (30770712, 30970889) and a research grant from The Joint PekingU-PolyU Center for Child Development and Learning. We thank the anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments and suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Xiaolin Zhou.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Meng, X., Cheng-Lai, A., Zeng, B. et al. Dynamic visual perception and reading development in Chinese school children. Ann. of Dyslexia 61, 161–176 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0049-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0049-2

Keywords

Navigation