Abstract
In this paper we address the usefulness of the notion of a paradigm in the context of derivational morphology. We first define a notion of paradigmatic system that extends conservatively the notion as it is used in inflection so as to be applicable to collections of structured families of derivationally-related words. We then build on this definition in an empirical quantitative study of derivational families of verbs in French. We apply information-theoretic measures of predictability initially designed by Ackerman et al. (2009) in the context of inflection. We conclude that key quantitative properties are common to inflectional and derivational paradigmatic systems, and hence that (partial) paradigms are an important ingredient of the study of derivation.



Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
For readability we use ‘derivational family’ as a shortcut for ‘morphological family of derivationally-related words’. See Sect. 2 for more precise definitions.
Although we will use a bit of mathematical notation for the sake of explicitness, the following obviously cannot be taken to be a full formalization of the notion of a paradigmatic system.
See Strnadová (2014, chap. 4) for relevant discussion of measures of regularity of the morphological relation between two words. Strnadová argues that the generalisability of a pattern across word pairs is a more relevant measure than similarity between the two words, although some combination of these two criteria is probably optimal.
Taking families to be tuples rather than sets will be convenient when defining alignment below. Nothing however hinges on the order of the elements of the family.
Remember that our notion of content encompasses morphosyntactic properties: hence two words may be strict synonyms but still not have the same content. This is the case of contrasting forms of a lexeme differing only in the realization of contextual inflection, such as contrasting forms of French adjectives discussed above.
Note that this is exactly parallel to the way Bonami and Stump (2016) suggest to address overabundance in Paradigm Function Morphology.
Also note that combinations of suppletion and overabundance are also found in the context of inflection, as discussed in Grossman and Thornton (forthcoming), Thornton (forthcoming).
We rely on conventional orthography in the case of Czech, since it is transparent enough. For French we use phonemic transcriptions, as mute orthographic letters introduce confusion as to the nature of the patterns of alternation.
Note that the PCFP is formulated as a question of linguistic structure (what licences inferences) rather than a question of psycholinguistic processing (what inferences are actually drawn by speakers). There is a long tradition of addressing the psycholinguistic question: wug tests and morphological priming tasks can both be seen as assessing the role of paradigmatic inferences in processing. However, the research questions in the service of which these tasks have been put often have little to do with the PCFP. In this paper we focus on the linguistic issue and on methods stemming from Ackerman et al.’s seminal paper.
This can already be seen using the toy dataset in Table 5. Computations parallel to the ones in the preceding section reveal that predicting the m.sg from the m.pl is harder than the other way around: H(m.sg ⇒ m.pl)\({}= \frac{3}{4}\log_{2} 3 - \frac{1}{2} \approx0.69\). This is due to the fact that in this dataset, there are more instances where a m.pl has a form that does not fully determine that of its m.sg than the other way around.
To see how this works intuitively, consider again the toy dataset in Table 5, and prediction of the m.sg from joint knowledge of the f.sg and m.pl. Predicting the pattern relating m.sg and m.pl (i.e., the value of ‘m.sg ∼ m.pl’ on the basis of just the m.pl is hard, because plurals in /o/ may correspond to two kinds of singulars; that is, there is one value of ‘m.plm.sg∼m.pl’ which corresponds to two possible values of ‘m.sg ∼ m.pl’. However, if we add in knowledge of the relationship between m.pl and f.sg, no uncertainty remains: all lexemes with an Xal ∼ Xo alternation between m.sg and m.pl exhibit the same alternation between f.sg and m.pl.
If the 4 strategies were strictly equiprobable, the implicative entropy would be higher than 2, the entropy associated with choosing the value of a variable with 4 equiprobable outcomes.
The two formations also contrast in being matched with feminine nouns in -euse
or -rice
respectively.
Note that this pattern applies both in regular cases like formation∼formateur, where the Latinate stem itself is predictable from the verb’s basic stem, and in cases like correction∼correcteur, where the Latinate stem is unpredictable. This also contributes to making prediction of Agent nouns from Action nouns easy: Agent and Action nouns do not contrast in terms of unpredictable patterns of allomorphy.
There are only five exceptions in the dataset, including constateur ‘in charge of reporting’ related to constater ‘take notice’.
References
Ackerman, F., & Malouf, R. (2013). Morphological organization: the low conditional entropy conjecture. Language, 89, 429–464.
Ackerman, F., Blevins, J. P., & Malouf, R. (2009). Parts and wholes: implicative patterns in inflectional paradigms. In J. P. Blevins & J. Blevins (Eds.), Analogy in grammar (pp. 54–82). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Albright, A. C. (2002). The identification of bases in morphological paradigms. PhD thesis, University of California, Los Angeles.
Anderson, S. R. (1992). A-morphous morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Aronoff, M. (1994). Morphology by itself. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Baerman, M., Brown, D., & Corbett, G. G. (2005). The syntax–morphology interface: a study of syncretism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Baerman, M., Corbett, G. G., & Brown, D. (Eds.) (2010). Defective paradigms. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baroni, M., Bernardini, S., Ferraresi, A., & Zanchetta, E. (2009). The wacky wide web: a collection of very large linguistically processed web-crawled corpora. In Language resources and evaluation (Vol. 43, pp. 209–226).
Bauer, L. (1997). Derivational paradigms. In G. Booij & J. van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1996 (pp. 243–256). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Becker, T. (1993). Back-formation, cross-formation, and ‘bracketing paradoxes’ in paradigmatic morphology. In G. Booij & J. van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1993 (pp. 1–25). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Behrens, L. (1995). Lexical rules cross-cutting inflection and derivation. Acta Linguistica Hungarica, 43(1/2), 33–65.
Blevins, J. P. (2001). Paradigmatic derivation. Transactions of the Philological Society, 99(2), 211–222.
Blevins, J. P. (2016). Word and paradigm morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bochner, H. (1993). Simplicity in generative morphology. Berlin: de Gruyter.
Bonami, O., & Beniamine, S. (2016). Joint predictiveness in inflectional paradigms. Word Structure, 9(2), 156–182.
Bonami, O., & Boyé, G. (2002). Suppletion and stem dependency in inflectional morphology. In F. Van Eynde, L. Hellan, & D. Beerman (Eds.), The proceedings of the HPSG ’01 conference (pp. 51–70). Stanford: CSLI Publications.
Bonami, O., & Boyé, G. (2005). Construire le paradigme d’un adjectif. Recherches Linguistiques de Vincennes, 34, 77–98.
Bonami, O., & Boyé, G. (2006). Subregular defaults in French conjugation. In 12th international morphology meeting, Budapest.
Bonami, O., & Boyé, G. (2014). De formes en thèmes. In F. Villoing, S. Leroy, & S. David (Eds.), Foisonnements morphologiques. Etudes en hommage à Françoise Kerleroux (pp. 17–45). Presses Universitaires de Paris Ouest.
Bonami, O., & Luís, A. R. (2014). Sur la morphologie implicative dans la conjugaison du portugais: une étude quantitative. In J. L. Léonard (Ed.), Morphologie flexionnelle et dialectologie romane. Typologie(s) et modélisation(s), no. 22 in Mémoires de la Société de Linguistique de Paris (pp. 111–151). Leuven: Peeters.
Bonami, O., & Stump, G. T. (2016). Paradigm function morphology. In A. Hippisley & G. T. Stump (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of morphology (pp. 449–481). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bonami, O., Boyé, G., & Kerleroux, F. (2009). L’allomorphie radicale et la relation flexion-construction. In B. Fradin, F. Kerleroux, & M. Plénat (Eds.), Aperçus de morphologie du français (pp. 103–125). Saint-Denis: Presses de l’Université de Vincennes.
Bonami, O., Boyé, G., & Tseng, J. (2014a). An integrated analysis of French liaison. In P. Monachesi, G. Jäger, G. Penn, & S. Wintner (Eds.), Proceedings of the 9th conference on formal grammar (pp. 29–45). Stanford: CSLI Publications.
Bonami, O., Caron, G., & Plancq, C. (2014b). Construction d’un lexique flexionnel phonétisé libre du français. In F. Neveu, P. Blumenthal, L. Hriba, A. Gerstenberg, J. Meinschaefer, & S. Prévost (Eds.), Actes du quatrième congrès mondial de linguistique française (pp. 2583–2596).
Booij, G. (1997). Autonomous morphology and paradigmatic relations. In G. Booij & J. van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1996 (pp. 35–53). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Booij, G. (2010). Construction morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Boyé, G. (2006). Suppletion. In K. Brown (Ed.), The encyclopedia of language and linguistics (2nd ed., pp. 297–299). Oxford: Elsevier.
Boyé, G., & Schalchli, G. (2016). The status of paradigms. In A. Hippisley & G. Stump (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of morphology (pp. 206–234). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Carstairs, A. (1987). Allomorphy in inflection. London: Croom Helm.
Corbett, G. G. (2007). Canonical typology, suppletion and possible words. Language, 83, 8–42.
Cotterell, R., Vylomova, E., Khayrallah, H., Kirov, C., & Yarowsky, D. (2017). Paradigm completion for derivational morphology. arXiv:170809151.
Dressler, W. U. (1985). Suppletion in word formation. In J. Fisiak (Ed.), Historical semantics, historical word formation (pp. 97–112). Berlin: de Gruyter.
Fradin B. (forthcoming). Competition in derivation: what can we learn from duplicates? In: F. Gardani, H. C. Luschützky, & F. Rainer (Eds.), Competition in Morphology, Springer, Berlin, u. L’Acquila
Grossman, M., Thornton, A. M. (forthcoming). Overabundance in Hungarian accusative pronouns. In B. Baldi, L. Franco, M. Grimaldi, & R. Lai (Eds.), Structuring variation in Romance linguistics and beyond. Studies in honour of Leonardo M. Savoia, Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Haspelmath, M., & Sims, A. (2010). Understanding morphology. London: Routledge.
Hathout, N., & Namer, F. (2014). Démonette, a French derivational morpho-semantic network. Linguistic Issues in Language Technology, 11(5), 125–168.
Hathout, N., Sajous, F., & Calderone, B. (2014). GLÀFF, a large versatile French lexicon. In Proceedings of LREC, 2014.
Hjelmslev, L. (1938). Essai d’une théorie des morphèmes. In Actes du quatrième congrès international de linguistes (pp. 140–151). Copenhague: Munksgaard.
Jackendoff, R. (1975). Morphological and semantic regularities in the lexicon. Language, 51, 639–671.
Jakobson, R. (1939). Signe zéro. In Mélanges de linguistique offerts à Charles Bally, Geneva: Georg et Cie. reprinted in Jakobson (1971).
Jakobson, R. (1971). Selected writings II. The Hague: Mouton.
de Jong N. H., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2000). The morphological family size effect and morphology. Language and cognitive processes, 15(4–5), 329–365.
Kilbury, J. (1992). Paradigm-based derivational morphology. In Konvens (Vol. 92, pp. 159–168). Berlin: Springer.
Lignon, S., & Namer, F. (2010). Comment conversionner les v-ion ? ou la construction de v-ionnerverbe par conversion. In Actes du 2eme congrès mondial de linguistique française (pp. 1009–1028).
Mansfield, J. (2016). Intersecting formatives and inflectional predictability: how do speakers and learners predict the correct form of Murrinhpatha verbs? Word Structure, 9(2), 182–214.
van Marle, J. (1984). On the paradigmatic dimension of morphological creativity. Dordrecht: Foris.
Matthews, P. (2001). A short history of structural linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Matthews, P. H. (1972). Inflectional morphology. A theoretical study based on aspects of Latin verb conjugation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Matthews, P. H. (1974). Morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Mel’čuk, I. A. (1976). On suppletion. Linguistics, 170, 45–90.
Mel’čuk, I. A. (1994). Suppletion. Studies in Language, 18, 339–410.
Pirrelli, V., & Federici, S. (1994). Derivational paradigms in morphonology. In Proceedings of the 15th conference on computational linguistics (pp. 234–240).
Pirrelli, V., & Yvon, F. (1999). The hidden dimension: a paradigmatic view of data-driven NLP. Journal of Experimental & Theoretical Artificial Intelligence, 11(3), 391–408.
Plag, I. (1999). Morphological productivity. Berlin: de Gruyter.
Pounder, A. (2000). Process and paradigms in word-formation morphology (Vol. 131). Berlin: de Gruyter.
del Prado Martín F. M., Deutsch, A., Frost, R., Schreuder, R., Jong, N. H. D., & Baayen, R. H. (2005). Changing places: a cross-language perspective on frequency and family size in Dutch and Hebrew. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(4), 496–512.
Robins, R. H. (1959). In defense of WP. Transactions of the Philological Society, 116–144.
Saussure, F. (1916). Cours de linguistique générale. Paris: Payot.
Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (1997). How complex simple words can be. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 118–139.
Sims, A. (2015). Inflectional defectiveness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Strnadová, J. (2014). Les réseaux adjectivaux: Sur la grammaire des adjectifs dénominaux en français. PhD thesis, Université Paris Diderot et Univerzita Karlova V Praze.
Stump, G. T. (1998). Inflection. In A. Spencer & A. Zwicky (Eds.), The handbook of morphology (pp. 13–43). London: Blackwell.
Stump, G. T. (2001). Inflectional morphology. A theory of paradigm structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Stump, G. T. (2005). Referrals and morphomes in Sora verb inflection. In G. Booij & J. van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology 2005 (pp. 227–251). Dordrecht: Springer.
Stump, G. T. (2006). Heteroclisis and paradigm linkage. Language, 82, 279–322.
Stump, G. T. (2016). Inflectional paradigms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Stump, G. T., & Finkel, R. (2013). Morphological typology: from word to paradigm. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Thornton, A. M. (2011). Overabundance (multiple forms realizing the same cell): a non-canonical phenomenon in Italian verb morphology. In M. Maiden, J. C. Smith, M. Goldbach, & M. O. Hinzelin (Eds.), Morphological autonomy: perspectives from romance inflectional morphology (pp. 358–381). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Thornton, A. M. (2012). Reduction and maintenance of overabundance. a case study on Italian verb paradigms. Word Structure, 5, 183–207.
Thornton, A. M. (forthcoming) Overabundance: a canonical typology. In F. Rainer, F. Gardani, H. C. Luschützky, & W. U. Dressler (Eds.), Competition in morphology, Dordrecht: Springer.
Tribout, D. (2010). How many conversions from verb to noun are there in French? In Proceedings of the HPSG 2010 conference (pp. 341–357). Stanford: CSLI Publications.
Štekauer, P. (2014). Derivational paradigms. In R. Lieber & P. Štekauer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of derivational morphology (pp. 354–369). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Wunderlich, D., & Fabri, R. (1995). Minimalist morphology: an approach to inflection. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 14(2), 236–294.
Wurzel, W. U. (1984). Flexionsmorphologie und Natürlichkeit. Ein Beitrag zur morphologischen Theoriebildung. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag, translated as Wurzel (1989).
Wurzel, W. U. (1989). Inflectional morphology and naturalness. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Acknowledgements
Aspects of this work were presented at the Workshop on Paradigms in Word Formation (Naples, September 2016), at the first ParadigMo conference (Toulouse, June 2017), and at Université Paris Diderot. We thank the audiences at these events, and in particular Laurie Bauer, Sacha Beniamine, Gilles Boyé, Nabil Hathout, Fiammetta Namer, Andrew Spencer, and Delphine Tribout, for their comments and suggestions. We also thank Farrell Ackerman and Anna M. Thornton, two anonymous reviewers, and the guest editors for this special issue for useful suggestions. This work was partially supported by a public grant overseen by the French National Research Agency (ANR) as part of the “Investissements d’Avenir” program (reference: ANR-10-LABX-0083).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Bonami, O., Strnadová, J. Paradigm structure and predictability in derivational morphology. Morphology 29, 167–197 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-018-9322-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-018-9322-6