Skip to main content
Log in

Tutor learning: the role of explaining and responding to questions

  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous research on peer tutoring has found that students sometimes benefit academically from tutoring other students. In this study we combined quantitative and qualitative analyses to explore how untrained peer tutors learned via explaining and responding to tutee questions in a non-reciprocal tutoring setting. In support of our hypotheses, we found that tutors learned most effectively when their instructional activities incorporated reflective knowledge-building in which they monitored their own understanding, generated inferences to repair misunderstandings, and elaborated upon the source materials. However, tutors seemed to adopt a knowledge-telling bias in which they primarily summarized the source materials with little elaboration. Tutors’ reflective knowledge-building activities, when they occurred, were more frequently elicited by interactions with their tutee. In particular, when tutees asked questions that contained an inference or required an inferential answer, tutors’ responses were more likely to be elaborative and metacognitive. Directions for future research are also discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen, V. L. (1983). Impact of the role of the tutor on behavior and self-perceptions. In J. Levine & M. Wang (Eds.), Teacher and student perception: Implications for learning (pp. 367–389). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, V. L., & Feldman, R. S. (1976). Studies on the role of tutor. In V. Allen (Ed.), Children as teachers: Theory and research on tutoring (pp. 113–129). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Annis, L. (1983). The processes and effects of peer tutoring. Human Learning, 2, 39–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R., Renkl, A., & Merrill, M. (2003). Transitioning from studying examples to solving problems: Effects of self-explanation prompts and fading worked-out steps. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 774–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Azevedo, R., & Hadwin, A. (2005). Scaffolding self-regulated learning and metacognition: Implications for the design of computer-based scaffolds. Instructional Science, 33, 367–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. (1979). Comprehension monitoring: Identifying and coping with text confusions. Journal of Reading Behavior, 11(4), 367–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Schul, Y. (1980). On the cognitive benefits of teaching. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 593–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benware, C. A., & Deci, E. L. (1984). Quality of learning with an active versus passive motivational set. American Educational Research Journal, 21(4), 755–765.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, B. S. (1984). The 2 sigma problem: The search for methods of group instruction as effective as one-to-one tutoring. Educational Researcher, 13, 4–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A., & Day, J. (1983). Macrorules for summarizing text: The development of expertise. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, C. (2001). Peer collaboration and discourse patterns in learning from incompatible information. Instructional Science, 29, 443–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. (1997). Quantifying qualitative analyses of verbal data: A practical guide. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 6(3), 271–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. (2000). Self-explaining expository texts: The dual process of generating inferences and repairing mental models. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (pp. 161–238). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M., Siler, S., & Jeong, H. (2004). Can tutors monitor students understanding accurately? Cognition and Instruction, 22(3), 363–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M., Bassok, M., Lewis, M., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13, 145–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M., deLeeuw, N., Chiu, M., & LaVancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanation improves understanding. Cognitive Science, 18, 439–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M., Siler, S., Jeong, H., Yamauchi, T., & Hausmann, R. G. (2001). Learning from human tutoring. Cognitive Science, 25, 471–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1986). Theoretical considerations of peer tutoring. Psychology in the Schools, 23, 175–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, P. A., Kulik, J. A., & Kulik, C. C. (1982). Educational outcomes of tutoring: A meta-analysis of findings. American Educational Research Journal, 19(2), 237–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, E. B. (1998). Using explanatory knowledge during collaborative problem-solving in science. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 7, 387–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, E. B., Brown, A. L., & Rivkin, I. D. (1997). The effect of instructional explanations on formal learning from scientific texts. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 6(4), 347–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, E. (2003). Prompting middle school science students for productive reflection: Generic and directed prompts. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 12(1), 91–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derry, S. J., & Potts, M. K. (1998). How tutors model students: A study of personal constructs in adaptive tutoring. American Educational Research Journal, 35(1), 65–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duit, R., Roth, W., Komorek, M., & Wilbers, J. (2001). Fostering conceptual change by analogies—between Scylla and Charybdis. Learning and Instruction, 11, 283–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fantuzzo, J. W., Riggio, R. E., Connelly, S., & Dimeff, L. A. (1989). Effects of reciprocal peer tutoring on academic achievement and psychological adjustment: A componential analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(2), 173–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fogarty, J., & Wang, M. (1982). An investigation of the cross-age peer tutoring process: Some implications for instructional design and motivation. Elementary School Journal, 82(5), 450–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foot, H., Shute, R., & Morgan, M. (1997). Children’s sensitivity to lack of understanding. Educational Studies, 23(2), 185–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foot, H., Shute, R., Morgan, M., & Barron, A. (1990). Theoretical issues in peer tutoring. In H. Foot, M. Morgan, & R. Shute (Eds.), Children helping children. (pp. 65–92). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, S. (1996). Human physiology, 5th ed. Chicago: WCB Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Bentz, J., Phillips, N. B., & Hamlett, C. L. (1994). The nature of students’ interactions during peer tutoring with and without prior training and experience. American Educational Research Journal, 31, 75–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hamlett, C. L., Phillips, N. B., Karns, K., & Dutka, S. (1997). Enhancing students’ helping behavior during peer-mediated instruction with conceptual mathematical explanations. The Elementary School Journal, 97(3), 223–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gartner, A., Kohler, M. C., & Riessman, F. (1971). Children teach children: Learning by teaching. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenburg, A., Wilkinson, A., & Epstein, W. (1982). The illusion of knowing: Failure in the self-assessment of comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 10(6), 597–602.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, E. B. (1999). Sensation and perception. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., & McMahen, C. (1993). Anomalous information triggers questions when adults solve quantitative problems and comprehend stories. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(1), 136–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., & Person, N. K. (1994). Question asking during tutoring. American Educational Research Journal, 31(1), 104–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., Person, N. K., & Magliano, J. P. (1995). Collaborative dialogue patterns in naturalistic one-to-one tutoring. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 495–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwood, C. R., Carta, J. J., & Hall, R. V. (1988). The use of peer tutoring strategies in classroom management and educational instruction. School Psychology Review, 17(2), 258–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacker, D. (1998). Self-regulated comprehension during normal reading. In D. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 165–191). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamaker, C. (1986). The effects of adjunct question on prose learning. Review of Educational Research, 56(2), 212–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ismail, H., & Alexander, J. (2005). Learning within scripted and non-scripted peer-tutoring session: The Malaysian context. Journal of Educational Research, 99(2), 67–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. (1996). Learning to learn from effective tutors. In L. Schauble & R. Glaser (Eds.), Innovations in learning: New environments for education (pp. 49–74). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, A. (1994). Guiding knowledge construction in the classroom: Effects of teaching children how to question and how to explain. American Educational Research Journal, 31(2), 338–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, A. (1998). Transactive peer tutoring: Distributing cognition and metacognition. Educational Psychology Review, 10(1), 57–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, A., Staffieri, A., & Adelgais, A. (1998). Mutual peer tutoring: Effects of structuring tutorial interaction to scaffold peer learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(1), 134–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leinhardt, G. (2001). Instructional explanations: A commonplace for teaching and location for contrast. In V. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching: 4th Ed. (pp. 333–357). Washington, DC: AERA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maki, R., Shields, M., Wheeler, A., & Zacchilli, T. (2005). Individual differences in absolute and relative metacomprehension accuracy. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(4), 723–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McArthur, D., Stasz, C., & Zmuidzinas, M. (1990). Tutoring techniques in algebra. Cognition and Instruction, 7(3), 197–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D., Louwerse, M., Cai, Z., & Graesser, A. (2005, January 1). Coh-Metrix version 1.4. Retrieved September, 19, 2006, from http://cohmetrix.memphis.edu.

  • Merrill, D., Reiser, B., Merrill, S., & Landes, S. (1995). Tutoring: Guided learning by doing. Cognition and Instruction, 13(3), 315–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osborne, R., & Wittrock, M. (1983). Learning science: A generative process. Science Education, 67(4), 489–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palincsar, A., & Brown, A. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1(2), 117–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peverly, S., & Wood, R. (2001). The effects of adjunct questions and feedback on improving the reading comprehension skills of learning disabled adolescents. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 25–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., Ghatala, E. S., Woloshyn, V., & Pirie, J. (1990). Sometimes adults miss the main ideas and do not realize it: Confidence in responses to short-answer and multiple-choice comprehension questions. Reading Research Quarterly, 25(3), 232–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., McDaniel, M., Turnure, J., Wood, E., & Ahmad, M. (1987). Generation and precision of elaboration: Effects on intentional and incidental learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(2), 291–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, R. (1987). Structuring and adjusting content for students: A study of live and simulated tutoring of addition. American Educational Research Journal, 24(1), 13–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (1995). Learning for later teaching: An exploration of mediational links between teaching expectancy and learning results. Learning and Instruction, 5, 21–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrbeck, C. A., Ginsburg-Block, M. D., Fantuzzo, J. W., & Miller, T. R. (2003). Peer-assisted learning interventions with elementary school students: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(2), 240–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roy, M. (2001). Comprehension and learning through multimedia: Integrative processing of text and illustrations. Doctoral dissertation, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.

  • Saulny, S. (2005, April 4). A lucrative brand of tutoring grows unchecked. The New York Times, p. A18.

  • Scruggs, T. E., & Osguthorpe, R. T. (1986). Tutoring interventions with special education settings: A comparison of cross-age and peer tutoring. Psychology in the Schools, 23, 187–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharpley, A. M., Irvine, J., & Sharpley, C. F. (1983). An examination of the effectiveness of a cross-age tutoring program in mathematics for elementary school children. American Educational Research Journal, 20(1), 103–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Meij, H. (1994). Student questioning: A componential analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 6(2), 137–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VanLehn, K., Siler, S., Murray, C., Yamauchi, T., & Baggett, W. (2003). Why do only some events cause learning doing human tutoring? Cognition and Instruction, 21(3), 209–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, N. (1989). Peer instruction, problem-solving, and cognition: Multidisciplinary perspectives. International Journal of Educational Research, 13, 21–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D., Wood, H., Ainsworth, S., & O’Malley, C. (1995). On becoming a tutor: Toward and ontogenetic model. Cognition and Instruction, 13(4), 565–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Support was provided by the University of Pittsburgh Office of Graduate Studies, and grants from the National Science Foundation to the Center for Interdisciplinary Research on Constructive Learning Environments (NSF 9720359, www.pitt.edu/-circle) and the Pittsburgh Science of Learning Center (NSF SBE-0354420, www.learnlab.org). The authors gratefblly acknowledge the assistance and suggestions of Marguerite Roy, Robert Hausmann, Stephanie Siler, Bibinaz Pirayesh, and anonymous reviewers.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rod D. Roscoe.

Appendix

Appendix

Human visual system text excerpts

The entire text is not reproduced here due to the length, but two excerpts are provide to offer a flavor of the writing style and level. These excerpts are chosen to align with two of the episode examples shared, so the reader can further appreciate how text information was used differently in knowledge-telling and knowledge-building episodes. Text Excerpt 1 (from p. 7 of our text) describes the lens, ciliary muscles, and the process of accommodation. Episode Excerpt C shows an example of knowledge-building based on this information. Text Excerpt 2 (from p. 16 of our text) describes the horizontal and amacrine cells of the retina. Episode Excerpt A shows an example of knowledge-telling based on this text.

  1. (1)

    Although most of the bending of the light, called refraction, is done by the cornea, some additional focusing is accomplished by varying the thickness of the lens. This process is called accommodation. In this process, the ciliary muscles tighten and thereby increase the refractive power, and thus the focusing power, of the eye. Accommodation enables us to keep an image on the retina sharp as we look at objects located at different distances.

  2. (2)

    There are two other types of retinal cells called horizontal cells and amacrine cells. Horizontal cells connect receptors to other receptors. Amacrine cells connect ganglion cells to other ganglion cells and bipolar cells to other bipolar cells. These cells do not transmit signals toward the brain, but instead transmit and pool signals laterally across the retina. Thus, neural signals flow both directly toward the brain and laterally across the retina before going to the brain. (from p. 16 of the text materials).

Definitions Test items

Thirteen components were targeted on the eyeball diagram: aqueous humor, blind spot, choroid, cornea, eyelid, fovea, iris, lens, optic nerve, pupil, retina, sclera, and vitreous humor. An additional 13 terms were included as filler terms: amygdala, capillary, cochlea, duodenum, femur, larynx, ligament, macula, microvillae, nephron, pyramidal cell, sciatic nerve, and valve. On the retina diagram, seven components were targeted: amacrine cell, bipolar cell, cone receptor, ganglion cell, horizontal cell, optic nerve, and rod receptor. An additional nine items were included as filler terms: aqueous humor, barorecptor, blind spot, duodenum, leukocyte, microvillae, nephron, optic chiasm, and pyramidal cell. On both diagrams, some filler terms were unrelated to the eye (e.g. femur) and others were related to the eye but not indicated on the diagram (e.g. macula).

Questions Test items

The following questions were used in the Questions Test. Questions are listed in the order they appeared; those labeled with an asterisk appeared only on the post-test.

  1. (1)

    Which receptors are responsible for color and detail vision in high levels of light?

  2. (2)

    How is light entering the eye adjusted and directed toward the retina?

  3. (3)

    What is meant by the term “pooling of receptors”?*

  4. (4)

    What are the most direct and least direct pathways that neural signals can take the reach the brain from the receptors?

  5. (5)

    What muscles are involved in the process of accommodation?*

  6. (6)

    Why is the fovea the most sensitive portion of the retina for detecting light patterns?

  7. (7)

    What is the iris? What function does it serve?

  8. (8)

    Color blindness is a disorder in which a person is unable to see colors. Some people with color blindness are “blind” to all colors, while others are “blind” to just one or two colors. What might be the problem underlying color blindness? How could a person be “blind” to just one or two colors?

  9. (9)

    What is refraction?

  10. (10)

    What are some possible reasons why a person may need glasses? How do glasses help to compensate for these problems?

  11. (11)

    Why do humans see better in the light than the dark?

  12. (12)

    What structure us responsible for over two-thirds of the eye’s focusing power?

  13. (13)

    Most people never notice the “blind spot” in their vision? Why don’t people notice?*

  14. (14)

    What different types of cells are bipolar cells connected to?

  15. (15)

    Some animals can see in the dark. How might their eyes be structured different than human eyes so that this is possible?

  16. (16)

    What is the blind spot? Where is it located?*

  17. (17)

    How are receptors, neurons, and blood vessels arranged in the retina?*

  18. (18)

    What are the two main purposes of the vitreous humor?*

  19. (19)

    Diabetes is an illness in which the body does not produce enough insulin (a hormone needed to metabolize sugar). If untreated, diabetes can sometimes lead to swollen blood vessel and blindness (along with other health problems. How could diabetes lead to blindness?*

  20. (20)

    What happens when a molecule of photopigment absorbs a photon?*

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Roscoe, R.D., Chi, M.T.H. Tutor learning: the role of explaining and responding to questions. Instr Sci 36, 321–350 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-007-9034-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-007-9034-5

Keywords

Navigation