Skip to main content
Log in

The Role of Genes and Environments in Linking the Need to Evaluate with Political Ideology and Political Extremity

  • Published:
Social Justice Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Understanding the origins of political ideology and political extremity at the individual level is becoming increasingly pressing in the face of polarization in the political domain. Building upon the motivated social cognition model of political ideology, we propose a motivated cognition approach to the study of political extremity with the need to evaluate as a key epistemic motive that contributes to political extremity. Moreover, we hypothesize that the link between the need to evaluate and political extremity may rest largely on shared genetic effects. This hypothesis builds upon existing biology and politics research, which has convincingly demonstrated that genes influence the direction of ideology, but has been largely silent on the role of genes in political extremity. To test our hypothesis, we consider several types of ideological, affective, and partisan extremity alongside conventional measures of political ideology and the need to evaluate in a behavioral genetic framework. Using a twin study methodology, we show for the first that the need to evaluate is heritable, that its phenotypic relationships with ideological extremity and strength are rooted in shared genetic influences, and, unexpectedly, that the relationship between the need to evaluate and some forms of political extremity is largely environmental. In examining the genetic and environmental components of the covariation of the need to evaluate with political ideology and right wing authoritarianism, we find limited support for shared genetic influences. Taken together, these results illustrate the value of adopting a biologically informed motivated cognition approach to the study of political ideology and political extremity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We conceive of these five aspects of political extremity as theoretically distinct, though they are positively intercorrelated. See also footnote 6.

  2. It is also possible to conduct a trivariate analysis to determine whether the need to evaluate accounts for genetic variance in political ideology or political extremity when controlling for other measures, such as the Big Five personality traits (cf. Ksiazkiewicz, Ludeke, & Krueger, 2016). The need to evaluate has modest phenotypic correlations with the Big Five personality traits. In our sample, the correlations are: openness (0.19), conscientiousness (0.02), extraversion (0.23), agreeableness (-0.09), emotional stability (0.01). Focusing on openness and extraversion, these two personality factors are largely unrelated to our measures of political extremity (all phenotypic correlations below 0.1 in absolute value). Openness does have significant phenotypic correlations with political ideology in these data (see Ksiazkiewicz, Ludeke, & Krueger, 2016), but controlling for openness does not substantively change the results discussed below. As such, we do not present analyses formally testing whether the need to evaluate accounts for unique variance in political ideology or political extremity when controlling for Big Five traits.

  3. Due to a labeling error, the middle response options on the need to evaluate scale were presented out of order. They were presented as “1 = Completely disagree, 2 = Moderately agree, 3 = Slightly agree, 4 = Slightly disagree, 5 = Moderately disagree, 6 = Completely agree.” This adds measurement error to our measure of the need to evaluate, which means that we likely have a more conservative estimate of the heritability of the need to evaluate than we might otherwise. Nonetheless, the scale demonstrates good reliability when coded as written (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.75, as compared to 0.87 in Jarvis & Petty, 1996). The reliability is considerably lower if we assume that participants fixed the poles (i.e., assumed that 1 and 6 were correct and that the intermediate categories were mislabeled; alpha = 0.56) or flipped the poles (assumed that 1 and 6 needed to be flipped; alpha = 0.56). For those participants that explicitly indicated that they were changing the labeling of the scale (e.g., by writing so on their response sheet), the participant response scale is used. Otherwise, we treat the responses as following the provided scale. This procedure produces a good Cronbach’s alpha (0.75).

  4. There is also a fourth component that is sometimes estimated to represented dominance genetic effects (D), but the simultaneous estimation of A, C, D, and E requires data beyond that provided by MZ and DZ twins.

  5. We focus here on the Wilson-Patterson measure of ideology, which contains less measurement error than the single-item measure (cf. Ksiazkiewicz, Ludeke, & Krueger, 2016).

  6. As discussed above, we think of each of the extremity measures as capturing one of five theoretically distinct forms of extremity: ideological extremity, ideological strength, ideological breadth, attitude bipolarity, and strength of partisanship. For this reason, we analyze the ten measures of extremity in these five groupings in the results. Nonetheless, these different aspects of extremity are related conceptually to each other (and empirically to the need to evaluate). When the ten measures are combined in an exploratory factor analysis, two significant factors emerge. After rotation, the first factor captures extremity in terms of the Wilson–Patterson measures. The second factor captures extremity of partisanship (extremity of party identification, extremity of feeling thermometers toward parties and candidates). The extremity of single-item ideology and the feeling thermometers toward political groups load somewhat on each factor. Both factors have a significant phenotypic correlation with the need to evaluate (0.26, 0.18) and significant unique environmental correlations with the need to evaluate (0.16, 0.21). Only Factor 1 has a significant genetic correlation (0.52; for Factor 2, rA = 0.06). We present these results here for the interested reader, but focus on the five types of extremity described previously in the main text.

  7. We kept these three types of feeling thermometers separate analytically because each represents a different type of political attitude theoretically (toward parties, candidates, and political movements). Nonetheless, the extremity of these attitudes is positively correlated. If we average together the three extremity measures, we do find a significant genetic correlation with the need to evaluate (0.30), in support of H3b, though we lose the unique environmental correlation (perhaps because the unique environmental factors that lead to attitude extremity toward parties, candidates, and political movements are not the same). We note this here simply to say that future research should utilize more nuanced measures for different types of attitudinal extremity to examine their relationships with the need to evaluate separately and jointly.

  8. A similar pattern holds for the direction of political ideology, especially economic ideology (which shares a sizeable genetic component with the need to evaluate), and right wing authoritarianism (for which the covariance with the need to evaluate is primarily through the unique environment). Here, too, research on how these traits co-develop could be theoretically insightful and practically useful for developing effective interventions for pursuing fairness and social justice, insofar as these attitudes are relevant to perpetuating existing hierarchies in society.

References

  • Alford, John, Funk, Carolyn, & Hibbing, John. (2005). Are political orientations genetically transmitted? American Political Science Review, 99(2), 153–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other ‘‘authoritarian personality’’. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 47–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amodio, D. M., Jost, J. T., Master, S. L., & Yee, C. M. (2007). Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1246–1247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ansolabehere, S., Rodden, J., & Snyder, J. M., Jr. (2008). The strength of issues: Using multiple measures to gauge preference stability, ideological constraint, and issue voting. American Political Science Review, 102(2), 215–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakker, B. N., & Lelkes, Y. (2016). Selling ourselves short? How abbreviated measures of personality change the way think about personality and politics. Working paper. Retrieved from http://www.bertbakker.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/01/Bakker_Lelkes_SellingOurselvesShort-1.pdf.

  • Barber, M., & McCarty, N. (2015). Causes and Consequences of Polarization. In N. Persily (Ed.), Solutions to political polarization in America (pp. 15–58). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, E., Schermer, J. A., & Vernon, P. A. (2009). The origins of political attitudes and behaviours: An analysis using twins. Canadian Journal of Political Science, 42(04), 855–879.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bizer, G. Y., Krosnick, J. A., Holbrook, A. L., Wheeler, S. C., Rucker, D., & Petty, R. E. (2004). The impact of personality on cognitive, behavioral, and affective political processes: The effects of the need to evaluate. Journal of Personality, 72, 995–1027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boker, S., Neale, M., Maes, H., Wilde, M., Spiegel, M., Brick, T., et al. (2011). OpenMx: An open source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrika, 76(2), 306–317.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & McGue, M. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences. Journal of Neurobiology, 54(1), 4–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carney, D. R., Jost, J. T., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Political Psychology, 29(6), 807–840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2010). Dynamic public opinion: Communication effects over time. American Political Science Review, 104, 663–680. doi:10.1017/S0003055410000493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party over policy: The dominating impact of group influence on political beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(5), 808–822. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Converse, Philip E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowson, H. M. (2009). Are all conservatives alike? A study of the psychological correlates of cultural and economic conservatism. The Journal of Psychology, 143(5), 449–463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., Peterson, E., & Slothuus, R. (2013). How elite partisan polarization affects public opinion formation. American Political Science Review, 107(1), 57–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & Leeper, T. J. (2012). Learning more from political communication experiments: Pretreatment and its effects. American Journal of Political Science, 56(4), 875–896.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dvir-Gvirsman, S. (2014). One-track minds? Cognitive needs, media diet, and overestimation of public support for one’s views. Media Psychology, 18(4), 475–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazekas, Z., & Littvay, L. (2015). The importance of context in the genetic transmission of US party identification. Political Psychology, 36(4), 361–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M. (2004). Predicting attitude extremity: The interactive effects of schema development and the need to evaluate—and their mediation by evaluative integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1281–1294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M. (2007). Expertise, evaluative motivation, and the structure of citizens’ ideological commitments. Political Psychology, 28, 535–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M., & Hunt, C. V. (2013). Political information, political involvement, and reliance on ideology in political evaluation. Political Behavior, 35, 89–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Federico, C. M., & Schneider, M. (2007). Political expertise and the use of ideology: Moderating effects of evaluative motivation. Public Opinion Quarterly, 71, 221–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, S. (2013). Political ideology. In L. Huddy, D.O. Sears, & J.S. Levy (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of political psychology. New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199760107.013.0019.

  • Feldman, S., & Johnston, C. (2014). Understanding the determinants of political ideology: Implications of structural complexity. Political Psychology, 35(3), 337–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friesen, A., & Ksiazkiewicz, A. (2015). Do political attitudes and religiosity share a genetic path? Political Behavior, 37(4), 791–818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerber, A. S., Huber, G. A., Doherty, D., Dowling, C. M., & Ha, S. E. (2010). Personality and political attitudes: Relationships across issue domains and political contexts. American Political Science Review, 104(1), 111–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hatemi, P. K., Funk, C. L., Medland, S. E., Maes, H. M., Silberg, J. L., Martin, N. G., et al. (2009). Genetic and environmental transmission of political attitudes over a life time. Journal of Politics, 71(3), 1141–1156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatemi, P. K., Gillespie, N. A., Eaves, L. J., Maher, B. S., Webb, B. T., Heath, A. C., et al. (2011). A genome-wide analysis of liberal and conservative political attitudes. Journal of Politics, 73(1), 271–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatemi, P. K., Medland, S. E., Klemmensen, R., Oskarsson, S., Littvay, L., Dawes, C. T., et al. (2014). Genetic influences on political ideologies: Twin analyses of 19 measures of political ideologies from five democracies and genome-wide findings from three populations. Behavior Genetics, 44(3), 282–294.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hatemi, P. K., & Verhulst, B. (2015). Political attitudes develop independently of personality traits. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0118106. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0118106.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hetherington, M. J., & Rudolph, T. J. (2015). Why Washington won’t work: Polarization, political trust, and the governing crisis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hetherington, M. J., & Weiler, J. D. (2009). Authoritarianism and polarization in American politics. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., & Alford, J. R. (2014). Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(3), 297–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh, J. B., DeYoung, C. G., Xu, X., & Peterson, J. B. (2010). Compassionate liberals and polite conservatives: Associations of agreeableness with political ideology and moral values. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 655–664.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2009). Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 714–725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D., Iyer, R., & Haidt, J. (2012). Disgust sensitivity, political conservatism, and voting. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(5), 537–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S., Sood, G., & Lelkes, Y. (2012). Affect, not ideology: A social identity perspective on polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76(3), 405–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2015). Fear and loathing across party lines: New evidence on group polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(3), 690–707.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, W. B. G., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The need to evaluate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 172–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T. (2006). The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist, 61(7), 651–670.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2009). Political ideology: Its structure, functions and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 307–337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339–375.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Kay, A. C., & Thorisdottir, H. (Eds.). (2009). Social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Kruglanski, A. W., & Simon, L. (1999). Effects of epistemic motivation on conservatism, intolerance and other system-justifying attitudes. In L. I. Thompson, J. M. Levine, & D. M. Messick (Eds.), Shared cognition in organizations: The management of knowledge (pp. 91–116). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Nam, H. H., Amodio, D. M., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2014). Political neuroscience: The beginning of a beautiful friendship. Political Psychology, 35, 3–42. doi:10.1111/pops.12162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Napier, J. L., Thorisdottir, H., Gosling, S. D., Palfai, T. P., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Are needs to manage uncertainty and threat associated with political conservatism or ideological extremity? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 989–1007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Nosek, B. A., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Ideology: Its resurgence in social, personality, and political psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(2), 126–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kandler, C., Bleidorn, W., & Riemann, R. (2012). Left or right? Sources of political orientation: The roles of genetic factors, cultural transmission, assortative mating, and personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 633–645.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kemmelmeier, M. (1997). Need for closure and political orientation among German university students. Journal of Social Psychology, 137, 787–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krosnick, J. A., Boninger, D. S., Chuang, Y. C., Berent, M. K., & Carnot, C. G. (1993). Attitude strength: One construct or many related constructs? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(6), 1132–1151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ksiazkiewicz, A., Ludeke, S., & Krueger, R. (2016). The role of cognitive style in the link between genes and political ideology. Political Psychology. doi:10.1111/pops.12318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leader Maynard, J., & Mildenberger, M. (2016). Convergence and divergence in the study of ideology: A critical review. British Journal of Political Science. doi:10.1017/S0007123415000654.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leeper, T. J. (2014). Cognitive style and the survey response. Public Opinion Quarterly, 78(4), 974–983.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ludeke, S. G., Johnson, W., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (2013). “Obedience to traditional authority:” A heritable factor underlying authoritarianism, conservatism and religiousness. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(4), 375–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malka, A., Soto, C. J., Inzlicht, M., & Lelkes, Y. (2014). Do needs for security and certainty predict cultural and economic conservatism? A cross-national analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 1031–1051.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, L. (2015). “I Disrespectfully Agree”: The differential effects of partisan sorting on social and issue polarization. American Journal of Political Science, 59(1), 128–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarty, N., Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (2006). Polarized America: The dance of ideology and unequal riches. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medland, S. E., & Hatemi, P. K. (2009). Political science, biometric theory, and twin studies: A methodological introduction. Political Analysis, 17(2), 191–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mondak, J. J. (2010). Personality and the foundations of political behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nir, L. (2011). Motivated reasoning and public opinion perception. Public Opinion Quarterly, 75(3), 504–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osborne, D., Wootton, L. W., & Sibley, C. G. (2013). Are liberals agreeable or not? Politeness and compassion differentially predict political conservatism via distinct ideologies. Social Psychology, 44, 354–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oskarsson, S., Cesarini, D., Dawes, C. T., Fowler, J. H., Johannesson, M., Magnusson, P. K. E., et al. (2015). Linking genes and political orientations: Testing the cognitive ability as media hypothesis. Political Psychology, 36(6), 649–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oxley, D. R., Smith, K. B., Alford, J. R., Hibbing, M. V., Miller, J. L., Scalora, M., et al. (2008). Political attitudes vary with physiological traits. Science, 321(5896), 1667–1670.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prior, M. (2013). Media and political polarization. Annual Review of Political Science, 16, 101–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redlawsk, D. P. (2002). Hot cognition or cool consideration? Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. Journal of Politics, 64, 1021–1044. doi:10.1111/1468-2508.00161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rico, G., & Jennings, M. K. (2016). The formation of left-right identification: Pathways and correlates of parental influence. Political Psychology, 37(2), 237–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sargent, M. J. (2004). Less thought, more punishment: Need for cognition predicts support for punitive responses to crime. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(11), 1485–1493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human values? Journal of Social Issues, 50(4), 19–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Settle, J. E., Dawes, C. T., & Fowler, J. H. (2009). The heritability of partisan attachment. Political Research Quarterly, 62(3), 601–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sibley, C. G., Osborne, D., & Duckitt, J. (2012). Personality and political orientation: Meta-analysis and test of a threat-constraint model. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(6), 664–677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sidanius, J. (1988). Political sophistication and political deviance: A structural equation examination of context theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 37–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K. B., Oxley, D., Hibbing, M. V., Alford, J. R., & Hibbing, J. R. (2011). Disgust sensitivity and the neurophysiology of left-right political orientations. PLoS ONE, 6(10), e25552. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025552.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stoker, L., & Bass, J. (2013). Political socialization: Ongoing questions and new directions. In G. C. Edwards, L. R. Jacobs, & R. Y. Shapiro (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of American public opinion and the media. New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199545636.003.0028.

  • Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terrizzi, J. A., Jr., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2010). Disgust: A predictor of social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 587–592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thurber, J. A., & Yoshinaka, A. (2016). American gridlock: The sources, character, and impact of political polarization. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhulst, B., Eaves, L. J., & Hatemi, P. K. (2012). Correlation not causation: The relationship between personality traits and political ideologies. American Journal of Political Science, 56(1), 34–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, G. D., & Patterson, J. R. (1968). A new measure of conservatism. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7(4), 264–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, X., Mar, R. A., & Peterson, J. B. (2013). Does cultural exposure partially explain the association between personality and political orientation? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1497–1517.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, X., Plaks, J. E., & Peterson, J. B. (2016). From dispositions to goals to ideology: Toward a synthesis of personality and social psychological approaches to political orientation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(5), 267–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zakrisson, I. (2005). Construction of a short version of the Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA) scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 863–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was funded, in part, by the Rice Social Science Research Institute. The remainder of the study was crowd-funded through the SciFund Challenge.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Aleksander Ksiazkiewicz.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Aleksander Ksiazkiewicz declares that he has no conflict of interest. Robert F. Krueger declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 56 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ksiazkiewicz, A., Krueger, R.F. The Role of Genes and Environments in Linking the Need to Evaluate with Political Ideology and Political Extremity. Soc Just Res 30, 381–407 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-017-0292-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-017-0292-3

Keywords

Navigation