Skip to main content
Log in

The Influence of the Relationship and Motivation on Inequity Aversion in Dogs

  • Published:
Social Justice Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

One crucial element for the evolution of cooperation may be the sensitivity to others’ efforts and pay-offs in comparison with one’s own costs and gains. If an individual responds to a disadvantageous reward distribution, then it would likely increase its relative fitness compared with those who do not (Brosnan & de Waal, Nature 428:140, 2004). Recent experimental research indicates that sensitivity toward unequal reward distribution is not a uniquely human trait; non-human primates react to inequity if they witness a conspecific that obtains a more attractive reward for the same effort. However, primates are not the only species that cooperate and thus would benefit from inequity aversion. Canids also frequently cooperate with group members (e.g., wolves, wild dogs, etc.) or humans (domestic dogs), allowing for the possibility that they also are sensitive to the reward distribution. We report the findings of two studies of dogs’ responses to inequity and the social factors that mediate such responses. In the first study, we investigated whether domestic dogs showed a response to the inequity of rewards received for the same action in pairs of familiar dogs. We found that dogs showed significant behavioral differences when tested without food rewards when in the presence of a rewarded partner compared to the baseline and asocial control situations. This indicated that the presence of a rewarded partner matters (Range, Horn, Virányi, & Huber, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106(1):340–345, 2009). In contrast to primates, dogs did not react to differences in the quality of food or effort. In the second study, we analyzed whether individual (motivational or personality) characteristics determined the response of each subject to unequal reward distributions or whether the subject’s responses depended on the specific relationship they had with its partner. We found that individual motivation and the quality of the relationship influenced the dogs’ performance in test conditions, but that these factors varied across the control and reward inequity conditions. Overall, our results suggest that inequity aversion in dogs is conditional on their and their partner’s rewards. Whether inequity aversion is based on the same mechanisms in both humans and non-human primates is still unclear.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alberts, S. C. (1994). Vigilance in young baboons: Effects of habitat, age, sex, and maternal rank on glance rate. Animal Behaviour, 47, 749–755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amsel, A. (1992). Frustration theory: Many years later. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 396–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Amsel, A., & Roussel, J. (1952). Motivational properties of frustration: I. Effect on a running response of the addition of frustration to the motivational complex. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, 363–368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff, M. (2004). Wild justice, cooperation and fair play: Minding manners, being nice, and feeling good. In R. Sussman & A. Chapman (Eds.), The origins and nature of sociality (pp. 53–79). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benus, R. F., den Daas, S., Koolhaas, J. M., & Oortmerssen, G. A. (1990). Routine formation and flexibility in social and non-social behaviour of aggressive and non-aggressive male mice. Behaviour, 112(3–4), 176–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, D. I. S., Judge, P. G., & Ruehlmann, T. E. (1993). Kinship, association, and social relationships in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). American Journal of Primatology, 31(1), 41–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boitani, L., & Ciucci, P. (1995). Comparative social ecology of feral dogs and wolves. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 7(1), 49–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonanni, R., Cafazzo, S., Valsecchi, P., & Natoli, E. (2010a). Effect of group size, dominance rank and social bonding on leadership behaviour in free-ranging dogs. Animal Behavior, 79, 981–991.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonanni, R., Valsecchi, P., & Natoli, E. (2010b). Pattern of individual participation and cheating in conflicts between groups of free-ranging dogs. Animal Behavior, 79, 957–968.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Are apes really inequity averse? Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 273, 3123–3128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Are apes inequity averse? New data on the token-exchange paradigm. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 175–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bräuer, J., & Hanus, D. (2012). Social Justice Research (forthcoming).

  • Brosnan, S. F. (2006). Nonhuman species’ reactions to inequity and their implications for fairness. Journal of Social Justice, 19, 153–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F. (2011). A hypothesis of the co-evolution of cooperation and responses to inequity. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5, 43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature, 425(6955), 297–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2004). Fair refusal by capuchin monkeys. Nature, 428, 140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Partial support from a nonreplication: Comment on Roma, Silberberg, Ruggiero, and Suomi. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(1), 74–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., Freeman, C., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Partner’s behavior, not reward distribution, determines success in an unequal cooperative task in capuchin monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 68(7), 713–724.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., Schiff, H. C., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Tolerance for inequity may increase with social closeness in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 1560, 253–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., Talbot, C., Ahlgren, M., Lambeth, S. P., & Schapiro, S. J. (2010). Mechanisms underlying responses to inequitable outcomes in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behavior, 79, 1229–1237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, J. R. A., du Toit, J. T., & Bingham, J. (2004). Free-ranging domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) as predators and prey in rural Zimbabwe: Threats of competition and disease to large wild carnivores. Biological Conservation, 115(3), 369–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cafazzo, S., Valsecchi, P., Bonanni, R., & Natoli, E. (2010). Dominance in relation to age, sex and competitive contexts in a group of free-ranging domestic dogs. Behavioral Ecology, 21(3), 443–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caine, N. G., & Marra, S. L. (1988). Vigilance and social organization in two species of primates. Animal Behavior, 36, 897–904.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capitanio, J. P. (1999). Personality dimensions in adult male rhesus macaques: Prediction of behaviors across time and situation. American Journal of Primatology, 47, 299–320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carder, B., & Berkowitz, K. (1970). Rats’ preference for earned in comparison with free food. Science, 167, 1273–1274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, M. S., & Grote, N. K. (2003). Close relationships. In T. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 447–461). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clutton-Brock, J. (1995). Origins of the dogs: Domestication and early history. In J. A. Serpell (Ed.), The domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour and interactions with people (pp. 199–216). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coppinger, R., & Schneider, R. (1995). Evolution of working dogs. In J. A. Serpell (Ed.), The domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour and interactions with people (pp. 21–47). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Courchamp, F., & Macdonald, D. W. (2001). Crucial importance of pack size in the African wild dog Lycaon pictus. Animal Conservation, 4, 169–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Creel, S., & Creel, N. M. (1995). Communal hunting and pack size in African wild dogs, Lycaon pictus. Animal Behavior, 50, 1325–1339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Creel, S., Creel, N. M., Mills, M. G. L., & Monfort, S. L. (1997). Rank and reproduction in cooperatively breeding African wild dogs: Behavioral and endocrine correlates. Behavioral Ecology, 8(3), 298–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, T. J., & Bekoff, M. (1989). Population and social biology of free-ranging dogs, Canis familiaris. Journal of Mammalogy, 70, 754–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Day, R. L., Coe, R. L., Kendal, J. R., & Laland, K. N. (2003). Neophilia, innovation and social learning: A study of intergeneric differences in callitrichid monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 65, 559–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M., & Luttrell, L. M. (1988). Mechanisms of social reciprocity in three primate species: Symmetrical relationship characteristics or cognition? Ethology and Sociobiology, 9(2–4), 101–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drent, P. J., & Marchetti, C. (1999). Individuality, exploration and foraging in hand-raised juvenile great tits. In N. J. Adams & R. H. Slotow (Eds.), Proceedings of the 22nd international ornithological congress, Durban (pp. 896–914).

  • Dubreuil, D., Gentile, M. S., & Visalberghi, E. (2006). Are capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) inequity averse? Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 273, 1223–1228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dugatkin, L. A. (Ed.). (1997). Cooperation among animals. Oxford series in ecology and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Erdohegyi, A., Topal, J., Virányi, Z., & Miklosi, A. (2007). Dog-logic: Inferential reasoning in a two-way choice task and its restricted use. Animal Behaviour, 74(4), 725–737.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Rockenbach, B. (2003). Detrimental effects of sanctions on human altruism. Nature, 422(6928), 137–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fontenot, M. B., Watson, S. L., Roberts, K. A., & Miller, R. W. (2007). Effects of food preferences on token exchange and behavioural responses to inequality in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Animal Behaviour, 74, 487–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, H. D., & Gosling, S. D. (2010). Personality in nonhuman primates: A review and evaluation of past research. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, S. D. (1998). Personality dimensions in spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112(2), 107–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, S. D., John, O. P., & Kwan, V. S. Y. (2003). A dog’s got personality: A cross-species comparative approach to personality judgements in dogs and humans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1161–1169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 439–444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, F. H., Mech, L. D., & Fritts, S. H. (1983). Pack size and wolf pup survival—Their relationship under varying ecological conditions. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 13(1), 19–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heinrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., et al. (2001). In search of homo economicus: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. American Economic Review, 91, 73–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, L. M., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Whiten, A. (2008). Observational learning in chimpanzees and children studied through ‘ghost’ conditions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 275, 835–840.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, A. (2012). Fair is fine, but more is better: Limits to inequity aversion in the domestic dog. Social Justice Research, 25. doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0158-7.

  • Koolhaas, J. M., Korte, S. M., De Boer, S. F., Van der Wegt, B. J., Van Reenen, C. G., Hopster, H., et al. (1999). Coping styles in animals: Current status in behaviour and stress-physiology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 925–935.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. F., Thompson, L., & Bazerman, M. H. (1989). Social utility and decision making in interpersonal contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(3), 426–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1974). Analogy as a source of knowledge. Science, 185, 229–234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Massen, J. J. M., van den Berg, L. M., Spruijt, B. M., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2011). Inequity aversion and the effect of relationship quality in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology. doi:10.1002/ajp.21014.

  • Mech, D. (1970). The wolf: The ecology and behaviour of an endangered species. Garden City, NY: Natural History Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mech, L. D. (1999). Alpha status, dominance, and division of labor in wolf packs. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie, 77(8), 1196–1203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mech, L. D., & Boitani, L. (2003). Wolf social ecology. In L. D. Mech & L. Boitani (Eds.), Wolves: Behavior, ecology, and conservation (pp. 1–35). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miklosi, A., Topal, J., & Csanyi, V. (2004). Comparative social cognition: What can dogs teach us? Animal Behaviour, 67, 995–1004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, H. C., Rayburn-Reeves, R., & Zentall, T. R. (2009). Imitation and emulation by dogs using a bidirectional control procedure. Behavioural Processes, 80, 109–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Naderi, S., Miklósi, Á., Dóka, A., & Csányi, V. (2001). Co-operative interactions between blind persons and their dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 74, 59–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pal, S. K. (2005). Parental care in free-ranging dogs, Canis familiaris. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 90(1), 31–47. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2004.08.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pal, S. K., Ghosh, B., & Roy, S. (1998). Agonistic behaviour of free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris) in relation to season, sex and age. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 59(4), 331–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pang, J.-F., Kluetsch, C., Zou, X.-J., Zhang, A.-b., Luo, L.-Y., Angleby, H., et al. (2009). mtDNA data indicate a single origin for dogs south of Yangtze River, less than 16,300 years ago, from numerous wolves. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 26(12), 2849–2864.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Papini, M. R. (2003). Comparative psychology of surprising nonreward. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 62, 83–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Papini, M. R., & Dudley, R. T. (1997). Consequences of surprising reward omissions. Review of General Psychology, 1, 175–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, J., & Bekoff, M. (2012). Wild justice redux: What we know about social justice in animals and why it matters. Social Justice Research, 25. doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0154-y.

  • Pongrácz, P., Vida, V., Bánhegyi, P., & Miklósi, Á. (2008). How does dominance rank status affect individual and social learning performance in the dog (Canis familiaris)? Animal Cognition, 11, 75–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Price, S. A., & Brosnan, S. F. (2012). To each according to his need? Variability in the responses to inequity in non-human primates. Social Justice Research, 25. doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0153-z.

  • Raihani, N. J., & McAuliffe, K. (2012). Does inequity aversion motivate punishment? Cleaner fish as a model system. Social Justice Research, 25. doi:10.1007/s11211-012-0157-8.

  • Range, F., Horn, L., Bugnyar, T., Gajdon, G. K., & Huber, L. (2009a). Social attention in keas, dogs, and human children. Animal Cognition, 12(1), 181–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Range, F., Horn, L., Virányi, Z., & Huber, L. (2009b). The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(1), 340–345.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Range, F., & Virányi, Z. (2011). Development of gaze following abilities in wolves (Canis lupus). PLoS ONE, 6(2), e16888. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016888.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Range, F., Virányi, Z., & Huber, L. (2007). Selective imitation in domestic dogs. Current Biology, 17(10), 868–872.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roma, P. G., Silberberg, A., Ruggiero, A. M., & Suomi, S. J. (2006). Capuchin monkeys, inequity aversion, and the frustration effect. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(1), 67–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savolainen, P., Zhang, Y. P., Luo, J., Lundeberg, J., & Leitner, T. (2002). Genetic evidence for an East Asian origin of domestic dogs. Science, 298(5598), 1610–1613.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, J. P., & Fuller, J. L. (1965). Genetics and the social behavior of the dog. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, B. (1980). The formation of relationships based on socio-spatial proximity in a free-ranging colony of rhesus monkeys. Primates, 21(4), 484–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, S. M., & Wilson, J. R. (1975). Behavioral and morphological evidence for binocular competition in the postnatal development of the dog’s visual system. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 161, 183–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, J. R., & Gilby, I. C. (2004). A conceptual, framework for nonkin food sharing: timing and currency of benefits. Animal Behaviour, 67, 603–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szetei, V., Miklosi, A., Topal, J., & Csanyi, V. (2003). When dogs seem to lose their nose: An investigation on the use of visual and olfactory cues in communicative context between dog and owner. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 83(2), 141–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tinklepaugh, O. L. (1982). An experimental study of representative factors in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 8, 197–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Topal, J., Gergely, G., Erdohegyi, A., Csibra, G., & Miklosi, A. (2009). Differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves, and human infants. Science, 325(5945), 1269–1272.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., & Wensing, J. A. B. (1987). Dominance and its behavioural measures in a captive wolf pack. In H. Frank (Ed.), Man and wolf (pp. 219–252). Dordrecht: Junk Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Wolkenten, M., Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Inequity responses of monkeys modified by effort. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 18854–18859.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verbeek, M. E. M., Boon, A., & Drent, P. J. (1996). Exploration, aggressive behaviour and dominance in pair-wise confrontations of juvenile great tits. Behaviour, 133, 945–963.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Virányi, Zs., & Range, F. (submitted). Wolves understanding of human communicative cues in a two choice task.

  • Wynne, C. D. L. (2004). Fair refusal by capuchin monkeys. Nature, 428, 140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto, S., & Takimoto, A. (2012). Social Justice Research (forthcoming).

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study has received research funding from the European Community’s Sixth Framework Program under contract number: NEST 012929, the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) project P21244-B17, Stiftung Aktion Österreich—Ungarn 74öu3, Royal Canin Ltd., and Christian Palmers. The authors thank Lydia Hopper, Sarah Brosnan, and an anonymous referee for their useful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript, and all the dogs and their owners for participating in our experiment.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Friederike Range.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Range, F., Leitner, K. & Virányi, Z. The Influence of the Relationship and Motivation on Inequity Aversion in Dogs. Soc Just Res 25, 170–194 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-012-0155-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-012-0155-x

Keywords

Navigation