Skip to main content
Log in

Sandhi-tone words prolong fixation duration during silent sentence reading in Chinese

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The current study examined whether or not lexical access is influenced by detailed phonological features during the silent reading of Chinese sentences. We used two types of two-character target words (Mandarin sandhi-tone and base-tone). The first characters of the words in the sandhi-tone condition had a tonal alternation, but no tonal alternation was involved in the base-tone condition. Recordings of eye movements revealed that native Mandarin Chinese readers viewed the base-tone target words more briefly than the sandhi-tone target words when they were infrequent. Such articulation-specific effects on visual word processing, however, diminished for frequent words. We suggest that a conflict in tonal representation at a character/morpheme level and at a word level induces prolongation in fixation duration on infrequent sandhi-tone words, and conclude that these tonal effects appear to reflect articulation simulation of words during the silent reading of Chinese sentences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. There is considerable debate on whether lexical tone is phonemic or not. Lexical tone is supra-segmental and many would also argue that it is phonemic, in the sense that a change in lexical tone modifies the lexical meaning in a systematic way. For better reconciliation with previous studies, we chose to use the term phonemic (or sub-phonemic) in the present study.

References

  • Abramson, M., & Goldinger, S. D. (1997). What the reader’s eye tells the mind’s ear: Silent reading activates inner speech. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1059–1068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J. (2010). Phonology is fundamental in skilled reading: Evidence from ERPs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 95–100. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.1.95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2005). The prosodic property of lexical stress affects eye movements in silent reading: Evidence from eye movements. Cognition, 96, B89–B100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J., & Rayner, K. (2004). Representing syllable information during silent reading: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 391–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J., Sanders, L. D., & Kingston, J. (2009). Skilled readers begin processing subphonemic feature by 80 ms during visual word recognition. Biological Psychology, 80, 84–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, J., Treiman, R., Kessler, B., & Rayner, K. (2006). Vowel processing during silent reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 32, 416–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., & Baayen, R. H. (2015). Parsimoniousmixed models. Available from arXiv:1506.04967 (stat.ME).

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, H., Franklin, S., & Howard, D. (2001). Age of acquisition and imageability ratings for a large set of words, including verbs and function words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 33, 73–79. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breen, M., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2011). Stress matters: Effects of anticipated lexical stress on silent reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 153–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers, E. (2018). The word frequency effect in word processing: An updated review. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(1), 45–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cao, J. (1986). Putonghua qingsheng yinjie texing fenxi. Applied Acoustics, 5(4), 1–6. (in Chinese).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based on film subtitles. PLoS ONE, 5(6), e10729. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H.-C., & Shu, H. (2001). Lexical activation during the recognition of Chinese characters: Evidence against early phonological activation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(3), 511–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies in information processing (pp. 151–216). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100, 589–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doctor, E. A., & Coltheart, M. (1980). Children’s use of phonological encoding when reading for meaning. Memory & Cognition, 8, 195–209. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vision Research, 43, 1035–1045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feng, G., Miller, K., Shu, H., & Zhang, H. (2001). Rowed to recovery: The use of phonological and orthographic information in reading Chinese and English. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 1079–1100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 263–279. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11001841.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godde, E., Bosse, M., & Bailly, G. (2020). A review of reading prosody acquisition and development. Reading and Writing, 33, 399–426. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09968-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hohenstein, S., & Kliegl, R. (2015). remef: Remove partial effects. [Computer software] Retrieved from https://github.com/hohenstein/remef/

  • Hoosain, R. (1991). Psycholinguistic implications for linguistic relativity: A case study of Chinese. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsieh, H. I. (1976). On the unreality of some phonological rules. Lingua, 38(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(76)90038-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huestegge, L. (2010). Effects of vowel length on gaze durations in silent and oral reading. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(5), 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inhoff, A. W. (1984). Two stages of word processing during eye fixations in the reading of prose. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 612–624. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(84)90382-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inhoff, A. W., Connine, C., & Radach, R. (2002). A contingent speech technique in eye movement research on reading. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34, 471–480. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kliegl, R., Masson, M. E. J., & Richter, E. M. (2010). A linear mixed model analysis of masked repetition priming. Visual Cognition, 18, 655–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kliegl, R., Nuthmann, A., & Engbert, R. (2006). Tracking the mind during reading: The influence of past, present, and future words on fixation durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 13–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, X., & Chen, Y. (2015). Representation and processing of lexical tone and tonal variants: Evidence from the mismatch negativity. PLoS ONE, 10(12), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143097.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, M., & Yan, J. (1980). Beijinghua qingsheng de shengxue xingzhi. Dialect, 3, 166–178. (in Chinese).

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, W., Inhoff, A. W., Ye, Y., & Wu, C. (2002). Use of parafoveally visible characters during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1213–1227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lukatela, G., Eaton, T., Sabadini, L., & Turvey, M. T. (2004). Vowel duration affects visual word identification: Evidence that the mediating phonology is phonetically informed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 30, 151–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luo, Y., Yan, M., Yan, S., Zhou, X., & Inhoff, A. W. (2016). Syllabic tone articulation influences the identification and use of words during Chinese sentence reading: Evidence from ERP and eye movement recordings. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(1), 72–92. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0368-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, J., Laubrock, J., & Yan, M. (2016). Parafoveal processing in silent and oral reading: Reading mode influences the relative weighting of phonological and semantic information in Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 1257–1273. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, J., Yan, M., Laubrock, J., & Shu, H. (2019). Lexical and sublexical phonological effects in Chinese silent and oral reading. Scientific Studies of Reading. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2019.1583232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model: Some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 112(1), 43–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Politzer-Ahles, S., & Zhang, J. (in press). Evidence for the role of tone sandhi in Mandarin speech production.Journal of Chinese Linguistics.

  • R Development Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/.

  • Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1457–1506. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210902816461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, R., & Radach, R. (2012). The dynamics of reading in non-Roman writing systems. Reading and Writing, 25, 935–950. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9369-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S. (1985). Constraining models of word recognition. Cognition, 20(2), 169–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., Waters, G. S., Barnes, M. A., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1984). When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23(3), 383–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implicatins for learning to read. Child Development, 74, 27–47. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, J.-L., Kliegl, R., & Yan, M. (2012). Parafoveal semantic information extraction in Traditional Chinese reading. Acta Psychologica, 141, 17–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.06.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, J.-L., Lee, C.-Y., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., & Yen, N.-S. (2004). Use of phonological codes for Chinese characters: Evidence from processing of parafoveal preview when reading sentences. Brain and Language, 91, 235–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C. (1987). A rows is a rose: Spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & Cognition, 15, 181–198. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, S.-Y. (1967). Phonological features of tone. International Journal of American Linguistics, 33, 93–105. https://doi.org/10.2307/1263953.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wickham, H. (2009). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wheat, K. L., Cornlissen, P. L., Frost, S. J., & Hansen, P. (2010). During visual word recognition, phonology is accessed within 100 ms and may be mediated by a speech production code: evidence from magnetoencephalography. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 5229–5233. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4448-09.2010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yan, M., Richter, E. M., Shu, H., & Kliegl, R. (2009). Chinese readers extract semantic information from parafoveal words during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 561–566. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.3.561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yan, M., & Sommer, W. (2019). The effects of emotional significance of foveal words on the parafoveal processing of N+ 2 words in reading Chinese sentences. Reading and Writing, 32, 1243–1256. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9914-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yan, M., Luo, Y., & Inhoff, A. W. (2014). Syllable articulation influences foveal and parafoveal processing of words during the silent reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 75, 93–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2014.05.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, J., Rayner, K., Li, N., & Wang, S. (2012). Is preview benefit from word n + 2 a common effect in reading Chinese? Evidence from eye movements. Reading and Writing, 25, 1079–1091. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9282-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, J., Wang, S., Tong, X., & Rayner, K. (2012). Semantic and plausibility effects on preview benefit during eye fixations in Chinese reading. Reading and Writing, 25, 1031–1052. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9281-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, C., Xia, Q., & Peng, G. (2015). Mandarin third tone sandhi requires more effortful phonological encoding in speech production: Evidence from an ERP study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 33, 149–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2014.07.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, W., Wang, A., Shu, H., Kliegl, R., & Yan, M. (2018). Word segmentation by alternating colors facilitates eye guidance in Chinese reading. Memory & Cognition, 46(5), 729–740. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0797-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2000). The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 1245–1265. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, Y. (1980). Pronunciations of phonetics within compound characters. Changchun: Jilin People’s Publisher. (original work in Chinese).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Yingyi Luo for her helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by Research Grants Council of Hong Kong Special Administrative Region, China (Project No. EdUHK ECS 28606818) and by Start-up Research Grant of University of Macau (SRG2019-00148-FSS).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ming Yan.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table

Table 3 Linear mixed model estimates for durations

3.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pan, J., Zhang, C., Huang, X. et al. Sandhi-tone words prolong fixation duration during silent sentence reading in Chinese. Read Writ 34, 841–857 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10093-7

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10093-7

Keywords

Navigation