Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Linguistic processing in Hebrew-speaking children from low and high SES backgrounds

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The aim of this study was to investigate the impact of Socio-Economic Status (SES) on Hebrew-speaking children’s developing ability to pluralize nouns and mark adjectives in agreement with them. Participants were 180 gradeschool children from mid-high SES and 180 peers from low SES, in six consecutive grade levels. The task consisted of 32 singular noun-adjective pairs with nouns classified into four categories by suffix type (Regular and Irregular) and by stem type (Nonchanging and Changing). Results showed a consistent advantage to the high SES children in accuracy of noun and adjective plurals, with gaps widening when the morphological requirements were harder, that is, in the irregular categories. Moreover, the fact that low SES children’s reaction times to producing the full plural phrase hardly decreased indicates that, unlike their high SES peers, they also did not gain more processing efficiency with age and schooling.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Stress is unmarked except in penultimate position, as final stress is default in Hebrew (Segall, Nir-Sagiv, Kishon-Rabin & Ravid, 2008).

  2. This refers only to non-root –t spelled ת (Ravid, 2005).

  3. A third agreement category of definiteness was not investigated in this paper.

  4. Or the whole sentence, in cases of predicative adjectives.

References

  • Anderson, A., & Stokes, S. (1984). Social and institutional influences on the development and practice of literacy. In H. Goelman, A. Oberg, & F. Smith (Eds.), Awakening to literacy (pp. 24–37). Exeter, NH: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D. (2005). Continuity in children’s literacy achievements: A longitudinal perspective from kindergarten to school. First Language, 25, 259–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D., & Biron, S. (2004). Joint storybook reading and joint writing interventions among low SES preschoolers: Differential contributions to early literacy. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 588–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D., Korat, O., & Levin, I. (2006). Maternal mediation in a young child’s writing activity: A sociocultural perspective. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 709–734). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D., & Levin, I. (2001). Mother-child joint writing in low SES: Sociocultural factors, maternal mediation and emergent literacy. Cognitive Development, 16, 831–852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arriaga, R. I., Fenson, L., Cronan, T., & Pethick, S. J. (1998). Scores on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of children from low- and middle-income families. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 209–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Au, K. (1998). Social constructivism and the school literacy learning of students of diverse backgrounds. Journal of Literacy Research, 30, 297–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Battin-Pearson, S., Newcomb, M. D., Abbott, R. D., Hill, K. G., Catalano, R. F., & Hawkins, J. D. (2000). Predictors of early high school drop-out: A test of five theories. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 568–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (1978). Modern Hebrew structure. Tel Aviv: University Projects Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (1981). Regularity vs. anomaly: The acquisition of inflectional morphology. Journal of Child Language, 8, 265–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (2008). The psycholinguistics of developing text construction. Journal of Child Language, 35, 735–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Ravid, D. (2010). Interpretation and recall of proverbs in three pre-adolescent populations. First Language, 30, 155–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., Nayditz, R., & Ravid, D. (2011). Linguistic diagnostics of written texts in two school-age populations. Written Language and Literacy, 14.

  • Bickel, B., & Nichols, J. (2007). Inflectional morphology. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, Volume III—Grammatical categories and the lexicon (2nd ed., pp. 169–240). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blachman, B. A., Tangel, D., Ball, E. W., Black, R., & Mcgraw, C. K. (1999). Developing phonological awareness and word recognition skills: A two-year intervention with low-income, inner-city children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 11, 239–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Black, E., Peppé, S., & Gibbon, F. (2008). The relationship between socio-economic status and lexical development. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 22, 259–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. (1995). Socioeconomic status differences in preschool phonological sensitivity and first grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 476–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 371–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn, J., & Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on children. Children and Poverty, 7, 55–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J. L. (1985). Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, F. A., & Ramey, C. T. (1994). Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement: A follow-up study of children from low-income families. Child Development, 65, 684–698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chevrot, J.-P., Nardy, A., & Barbu, S. (2011). Developmental dynamics of SES-related differences in children’s production of obligatory and variable phonological alternations. Language Sciences, 33, 180–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, M. M., & McBride-Chang, C. (2006). Gender, context, and reading: A comparison of students in 43 countries. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 331–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child Development, 70, 853–865.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Angiulli, A., Siegel, L. S., & Maggi, S. (2004). Literacy instruction, SES, and word-reading achievement in English-language learners and children with English as a first language: A longitudinal study. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 19, 202–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Angiulli, A., Herdman, A., Stapells, D., & Hertzman, C. (2008). Children’s event-related potentials of auditory selective attention vary with their socioeconomic status. Neuropsychology, 22, 293–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, L. (1976). Leshonam shel yeladim te’uney tipuax [The language of deprived children]. Studies in Education, 13, 133–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, W. (2000). Student engagement at school: A sense of belonging and participation. Retrieved from Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD). www.pisa.oecd.org.

  • Dressler, W. U. (1989). Prototypical differences between inflection and derivation. Zeitschrift für Phonetik, Sprachwissenschaft und Kommunikationsforschung, 42, 3–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duke, N. (2000). For the rich it’s richer: Print experiences and environments offered to children in very low- and very high-socioeconomic status first-grade classrooms. American Educational Research Journal, 37, 441–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. K. (1994). Economic deprivation and early childhood development. Child Development, 65, 296–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engel, P. M. J., Santos, F. H., & Gathercole, S. E. (2008). Are working memory measures free of socioeconomic influence? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 1580–1587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. J., Sherab, D. M., Savagea, J. H., Betancourta, L., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., et al. (2006). Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Research, 1110, 166–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, B. B. (1997a). Learning a new poem: Memory for connected speech and phonological awareness in low-income children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1285–1297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, B. B. (1997b). Memory for rote linguistic routines and sensitivity to rhyme: A comparison of low-income children with and without specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguists, 18, 345–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feitelson, D., & Goldstein, Z. (1986). Patterns of book ownership and reading to young children in Israeli school-oriented and nonschool oriented families. Reading Teacher, 39, 924–930.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fish, M., & Pinkerman, B. (2003). Language skills in low-SES rural Appalachian children: normative development and individual differences, infancy to preschool. Applied Developmental Psychology, 23, 539–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzgerald, J., Spiegel, D. L., & Cunningham, J. W. (1991). The relationship between parental literacy level and perceptions of emergent literacy. Journal of Reading Behavior, 23, 191–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., Schatschneider, C., Eakin, M. N., Fletcher, J. M., Moats, L. C., & Francis, D. J. (2006). The impact of instructional practices in Grades 1 and 2 on reading and spelling achievement in high poverty schools. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 31, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, V. C. M., & Hoff, E. (2007). Input and the acquisition of language: Three questions. In E. Hoff & M. Shatz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development (pp. 107–127). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gazmararian, J. A., Parker, R. M., & Baker, D. W. (1999). Reading skills and family planning knowledge and practices in a low-income managed-care population. Obstetrics and Gynecology, 93, 239–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginsborg, J. (2006). The effects of socio-economic status on children’s language acquisition and use. In J. Clegg & J. Ginsborg (Eds.), Language and social disadvantage: Theory into practice (pp. 9–27). London: Wiley Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hackman, D., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and brain development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 65–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harste, J., Burke, C., & Woodward, V. A. (1994). Children’s language and world: Initial encounters with print. In R. B. Ruddell, M. R. Ruddell, & H. Singer (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (4th ed., pp. 48–69). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. (Spring 2003). The early catastrophe: The 30 million-word gap. American Educator, Spring, 4–9.

  • Hart, B., & Risley, R. T. (1992). American parenting of language-learning children: Persisting differences in family-child interactions observed in natural home environments. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1096–1105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, R. T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74, 1368–1378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E., & Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. Journal of Communication Disorders, 38, 271–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C., Griffith, P. L., & Gough, P. B. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kishiyama, M. M., Boyce, W. T., Jimenez, A. M., Perry, L. M., & Knight, R. T. (2009). Socioeconomic disparities affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1106–1115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korat, O., & Levin, I. (2001). Maternal beliefs, mother-child interaction, and child’s literacy: Comparison of independent and collaborative text writing between two social groups. Applied Developmental Psychology, 22, 397–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laaha, S., Ravid, D., Korecky-Kröll, K., Laaha, G., & Dressler, W. U. (2006). Early noun plurals in German: Regularity, productivity, or default? Journal of Child Language, 33, 271–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lozoff, B., Jimenez, E., & Smith, J. B. (2006). Double burden of iron deficiency in infancy and low socioeconomic status: A longitudinal analysis of cognitive test scores to age 19 years. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 160, 1108–1113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lupien, S. J., King, S., Meaney, M. J., & McEwen, B. S. (2001). Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 653–676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J., & Xu, F. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57.

  • McCarthey, S. (1997). Connecting home and school literacy practices in classrooms with diverse populations. Journal of Literacy Research, 29, 143–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natriello, G., McDill, E., & Pallas, A. (1990). Schooling disadvantaged children: Racing against catastrophe. New York: Teachers’ College.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuman, S., & Celano, D. (2001). Access to print in low-income and middle-income communities. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 8–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ninio, A. (1980). Picture-book reading in mother-infant dyads belonging to two subgroups in Israel. Child Development, 51, 587–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nittrouer, S. (1996). The relation between speech perception and phonemic awareness: Evidence from low-SES children and children with chronic OM. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1059–1071.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noble, K. G., Norman, M. F., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8, 74–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, K. G., Wolmetz, M. E., Ochs, L. G., Farah, M. J., & McCandliss, B. D. (2006). Brain–behavior relationships in reading acquisition are modulated by socioeconomic factors. Developmental Science, 9, 642–654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (2005). Timing the brain: Mental chronometry as a tool in neuroscience. PLoS Biol, 3(e51), 204–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poverty Report. (2009). Administration of planning and research. Jerusalem, Israel: National Social Security Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, J. R., Roberts, J. E., & Jackson, S. C. (2006). Structural development of the fictional narratives of African American preschoolers. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 37, 178–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Purcell-Gates, V., & Dahl, K. L. (1991). Low-SES children’s success and failure at early literacy learning in skills-based classrooms. Journal of Reading Behavior, 23, 1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Qi, C. H., Kaiser, A. P., Milan, S., & Hancock, T. (2006). Language performance of low-Income African American and European American preschool children on the PPVT-III. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 37, 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raizada, R. D. S., & Kishiyama, M. M. (2010). Effects of socioeconomic status on brain development, and how cognitive neuroscience may contribute to levelling the playing field. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (1995). Language change in child and adult Hebrew: A psycholinguistic perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (2005). Hebrew orthography and literacy. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 339–363). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (2011). Spelling morphology: The psycholinguistics of Hebrew spelling. New York: Springer.

  • Ravid, D., Dressler, W. U., Nir-Sagiv, B., Korecky-Kröll, K., Souman, A., Rehfeldt, K., et al. (2008). Core morphology in child directed speech: Crosslinguistic corpus analyses of noun plurals. In H. Behrens (Ed.), Finding structure in data (pp. 25–60). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Levie, R. (2010). Adjectives in the development of text production: Lexical, morphological and syntactic analyses. First Language, 30, 27–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Schiff, R. (2006). Morphological abilities in Hebrew-speaking gradeschoolers from two socio-economic backgrounds: An analogy task. First Language, 26, 381–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Schiff, R. (2009). Morpho-phonological categories of noun plurals in Hebrew: A developmental study. Linguistics, 47, 45–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Schiff, R. (2011). From dichotomy to divergence: Number/gender marking on Hebrew nouns and adjectives across schoolage. Language Learning, 61.

  • Ravid, D., & Shlesinger, Y. (2001). Vowel reduction in Modern Hebrew: Traces of the past and current variation. Folia Linguistica, 35(3–4), 371–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raviv, T., Kessenich, M., & Morrison, F. J. (2004). Mediational model of the association between socioeconomic status and three-year-old language abilities: The role of parenting factors. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 528–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, M. E., & Fish, M. (2010). Language skills in low-SES rural Appalachian children: Kindergarten to middle childhood. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 238–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35, 185–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schieffer, J., & Busse, R. T. (2001). Low SES minority fourth-graders’ achievement. Research Bulletin, Vol. 6 (1). The Research Institute for Waldorf Education.

  • Schiff, R., & Korat, O. (2006). Sociocultural factors in children’s written narrative production. Written Language and Literacy, 9, 213–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiff, R., & Lotem, E. (2011). Social class differences in the development of reading ability: The role of phonological awareness and morphological awareness within two orthographies. First Language, 31, 131–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiff, R., & Ravid, D. (2004). Vowel representation in written Hebrew: Phonological, orthographic and morphological contexts. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 245–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiff, R., Ravid, D., & Levy-Shimon, S. (2011). Children’s command of plural and possessive marking on Hebrew nouns: A comparison of obligatory vs. optional inflections. Journal of Child Language, 38, 433–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzwald, O. R. (2001). Modern Hebrew. Munich: Lincom Europa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segall, O., Nir-Sagiv, B., Kishon-Rabin, L., & Ravid, D. (2008). Prosodic patterns in Hebrew child directed speech. Journal of Child Language, 35, 1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shatz, M., Diesendruck, G., Martinez-Beck, I., & Akar, D. (2003). The influence of language and socioeconomic status on children’s understanding of false belief. Developmental Psychology, 39, 717–729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (2009). Improving the numerical understanding of children from low-income families. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 118–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (Ed.). (1985). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (2001). Form-function relations: How do children find out what they are? In M. Bowerman & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 406–449). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Barnes, W. S., Chandler, J., Goodman, I. F., & Hemphill, L. (1991). Unfulfilled expectations: Home and school influences on literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, A. (1977). Lashon va-xashiva be-kerev talmidim te’uney tipuax be-yisrael (Language and thought in disadvantaged pupils in Israel). Tel Aviv: Otsar Ha-More.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, C., Lauinger, B., & Neville, H. (2009). Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: An event-related brain potential study. Developmental Science, 12, 634–646.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teale, W. H. (1986). Home background and young children’s literacy development. In W. H. Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.), Emergent literacy: Writing and reading (pp. 173–205). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tubul, M. (2003). Hitpalguam shel curaney ha-ribuy ba-ivrit ha-mikra’it ve-ha-mishna’it (The distribution of plural morphemes in Biblical and Mishnaic Hebrew). Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel.

  • White, M. J., & Kaufman, G. (1997). Language usage, social capital, and school completion among immigrants and native-born ethnic groups. Social Science Quarterly, 78, 881–912.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rachel Schiff.

Appendix

Appendix

32 Target nouns (singular/plural forms, gloss in singular)

  • Nonchanging stem, regular suffix, masculine

  • pil / pil-im ‘elephant’, xatul / xatul-im ‘cat’, tik / tik-im ‘bag’, agas / agas-im ‘pear’

  • Nonchanging stem, regular suffix, feminine

  • xulca / xulc-ot ‘shirt’, matana / matan-ot ‘present’, smixa / smix-ot ‘blanket’, sira / sir-ot ‘boat’

  • Nonchanging stem, irregular suffix, masculine

  • kinor / kinor-ot ‘violin’, olam / olam-ot ‘world’, sulam / sulam-ot ‘ladder’, sade / sad-ot ‘field’

  • Nonchanging stem, irregular suffix, feminine

  • beyca / beyc-im ‘egg’, shana / shan-im ‘year’, pnina / pnin-im ‘pearl’, nemala / nemal-im ‘ant’

  • Changing stem, regular suffix, masculine

  • tof / tup-im ‘drum’, cel / clal-im ‘shadow’, dli / dlay-im ‘bucket’, shor / shvar-im ‘bull’

  • Changing stem, regular suffix, feminine

  • taut / tauy-ot ‘error’, kalétet / kalat-ot ‘cassette’, dima / dma’-ot ‘tear’, misgéret / misgar-ot ‘frame’

  • Changing stem, irregular suffix, masculine

  • lev / levav-ot ‘heart’, kace / kcav-ot ‘edge’, iparon / efron-ot ‘pencil’, régesh / regash-ot ‘feeling’

  • Changing stem, irregular suffix, feminine

  • tolaat / tola’-im ‘worm’, dérex / drax-im ‘way’, ir / ar-im ‘city’, isha / nash-im ‘woman’

4 Target adjectives (masculine singular/plural/feminine plural)

  • tov / tovim / tovot ‘good’; gadol / gdolim / gdolot ‘big’; lavan / levanim / levanot ‘white’; shaket / shketim / shketot ‘quiet’

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Schiff, R., Ravid, D. Linguistic processing in Hebrew-speaking children from low and high SES backgrounds. Read Writ 25, 1427–1448 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9326-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9326-7

Keywords

Navigation