Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Roadmaps to Representation: An Experimental Study of How Voter Education Tools Affect Citizen Decision Making

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Efforts to educate citizens about the candidates and issues at stake in elections are widespread. These include distributing voter guides describing candidates’ policy views and interactive tools conveying similar information. Do these voter education tools help voters identify candidates who share their policy views? We address this question by conducting survey experiments that randomly assign a nonpartisan voter guide, political party endorsements, a spatial map showing voters their own and the candidates’ ideological positions, or both a spatial map and party endorsements. We find that each type of information strengthens the relationship between voters’ policy views and those of the candidates they choose. These effects are largest for uninformed voters. When spatial maps and party endorsements send conflicting signals, many voters choose candidates with more similar policy views, against their party’s recommendation. These results contribute to debates about citizen competence and demonstrate the efficacy of practical efforts to inform electorates.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Like Lupia (1994), we distinguish extensive fact-based information (encyclopedic) about candidates’ policy views from recommendations of knowledgeable information providers (shortcuts) that are easily acquired and allow voters to infer the consequences of their choices.

  2. Kuklinski et al. (2001) and Boudreau (2009) experimentally manipulate information in surveys or in laboratory settings. Lupia (1994) examines whether voters who know an information shortcut make different choices than voters who do not in a real-world direct democracy election.

  3. Other studies examine voter guides’ effects on turnout (Bedolla and Michelson 2012), ballot initiative outcomes (Rogers and Middleton 2015), or candidate evaluations (Lodge et al. 1995).

  4. Previously, we examined the effects of a randomly-assigned voter guide relative to a control group receiving no information. Here, we build on that design by comparing the effects of two information shortcuts—party endorsements and spatial maps (both separately and together)—with the voter guide. We assess the effects of information on: (1) informed versus uninformed voters and (2) voters whose policy views and partisanship are aligned versus at odds.

  5. The Republican Party’s webpage recommended “not Eric Mar” as a means of endorsing Lee without setting him up for a backlash from an overwhelmingly Democratic electorate.

  6. 202 of these letters were “returned to sender” by the Post Office.

  7. The other 185 surveys were for a separate study. The number of observations in our analyses is 344 after excluding those who fail to indicate a party or a preference between Lee and Mar.

  8. 52% of respondents reported spending “1–5 minutes” viewing the guide, while another 36% spent longer. 95% found the voter guide to be “somewhat” or “very helpful.”

  9. Bridging the profiles with these candidate and voter responses enhances the precision of the estimated ideal points, making it more likely that they reflect respondents’ true policy views.

  10. See the OA for randomization checks and models that include control variables.

  11. We also omit Ideology, as the variable Control * Ideology is coded to take the value of the respondent’s ideal point for respondents in the control group and zero otherwise.

  12. We used the pscl package in R to analyze candidate and voter responses to 65 policy questions. We estimated a one-dimensional model with uninformative priors for all model parameters. The first dimension correctly classifies 75.1% of candidate and voter responses. Adding a second dimension did not improve the model (see the OA for details).

  13. See the OA for models with an alternative measure of political knowledge.

  14. A naïve spatial model with no partisan bias would predict that among respondents with ideal points equidistant between Mar and Lee, support for Lee would be 0.50. We observe support levels significantly below 0.50, suggesting some bias in favor of Mar, the incumbent.

  15. We observe this effect despite the fact that our model includes Republicans and Independents. In the OA, we show that this effect is magnified when we exclude these respondents.

  16. Specifically, the effect of receiving a voter education tool on the effect of Ideology among low-knowledge respondents is 0.39 higher than among high-knowledge respondents (p < 0.05). The difference in the effects of the voter guide (0.35), spatial map (0.36), and party + map (0.51) is also significant (p < 0.10) and the difference in the effect of party cues (0.31) nearly so.

References

  • Ahn, T. K., Huckfeldt, R., & Ryan, J. B. (2014). Experts, activists, and democratic politics: Are electorates self-educating?. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arceneaux, K. (2008). Can partisan cues diminish democratic accountability? Political Behavior,30, 139–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arceneaux, K., & Kolodny, R. (2009). Educating the least informed: Group endorsements in a grassroots campaign. American Journal of Political Science,53(4), 755–770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bafumi, J., & Herron, M. C. (2010). Leapfrog representation and extremism: A study of American voters and their members in congress. American Political Science Review,104(3), 519–542.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, L. M. (1996). Uninformed voters: Information effects in presidential elections. American Journal of Political Science,40, 194–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bedolla, L. G., & Michelson, M. (2012). Mobilizing inclusion: Transforming the electorate through get-out-the-vote campaigns. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, D. (1948). On the rationale of group decision-making. Journal of Political Economy,56, 23–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudreau, C. (2009). Closing the gap: When do cues eliminate differences between sophisticated and unsophisticated citizens? Journal of Politics,71(3), 964–976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudreau, C., Elmendorf, C. S., & MacKenzie, S. A. (2015a). Lost in space? Information shortcuts, spatial voting, and local government representation. Political Research Quarterly,68(4), 843–855.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudreau, C., Elmendorf, C. S., & MacKenzie, S. A. (2015b). Informing electorates via election law: An experimental study of partisan endorsements and nonpartisan voter guides in local elections. Election Law Journal,14(1), 2–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudreau, C., & MacKenzie, S. A. (2014). Informing the electorate? How party cues and policy information affect public opinion about initiatives. American Journal of Political Science,58(1), 48–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, J. (2011). Elite influence on public opinion in an informed electorate. American Political Science Review,105(3), 496–515.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller, W., & Stokes, D. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpini, D., Michael, X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why it matters. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007). Framing public opinion in competitive democracies. American Political Science Review,101(4), 637–655.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clinton, J. D., Jackman, S., & Rivers, D. (2004). The statistical analysis of roll call data. American Political Science Review,98, 355–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party over policy: The dominating impact of group influence on political beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,85, 808–822.

    Google Scholar 

  • Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent (pp. 206–261). New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enelow, J. M., & Hinich, M. J. (1984). The spatial theory of voting. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garzia, D., Trechsel, A. H., Vassil, K., & Dinas, E. (2013). Indirect campaigning—Past, present and future of voting advice applications. In B. Grofman, A. H. Trechsel, & M. Franklin (Eds.), The Internet and Democracy in Global Perspective. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, D., Palmquist, B., & Schickler, E. (2002). Partisan hearts and minds: Political parties and the social identities of voters. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grofman, B., & Norrander, B. (1990). Efficient use of reference group cues in a single dimension. Public Choice,64, 213–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jessee, S. A. (2010). Partisan bias, political information and spatial voting in the 2008 presidential election. Journal of Politics,72(2), 327–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahan, D. M. (2013). Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection. Judgment and Decision Making,8(4), 407–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuklinski, J. H., Quirk, P. J., Jerit, J., & Rich, R. F. (2001). The political environment and citizen competence. American Journal of Political Science,45(2), 410–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (2001). Advantages and disadvantages of cognitive heuristics in political decision making. American Journal of Political Science,45(4), 951–971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lodge, M., Steenbergen, M. R., & Brau, S. (1995). The responsive voter: Campaign information and the dynamics of candidate evaluation. American Political Science Review,89(2), 309–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupia, A. (1994). Shortcuts versus encyclopedias: Information and voting behavior in california insurance reform elections. American Political Science Review,88, 63–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupia, A., & McCubbins, M. D. (1998). The democratic dilemma: Can citizens learn what they need to know?. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKelvey, R. D., & Ordeshook, P. C. (1986). Information, electoral equilibria, and the democratic ideal. Journal of Politics,8, 909–937.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mummolo, J., & Peterson, E. (2017). How content preferences limit the reach of voting aids. American Politics Research,45(2), 159–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, S. P. (2011). Dominating cues and the limits of elite influence. Journal of Politics,73(4), 1165–1177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, M. B., Slothuus, R., & Togeby, L. (2010). Political parties and value consistency in public opinion formation. Public Opinion Quarterly,74(3), 530–550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rahn, W. M. (1993). The role of partisan stereotypes in information processing about political candidates. American Journal of Political Science,37, 472–496.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, T., & Middleton, J. (2015). Are ballot initiative outcomes influenced by the campaigns of independent groups? Political Behavior,37(3), 567–593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shor, B., & Rogowski, J. C. (2016). Ideology and the US congressional vote. Political Science Research and Methods. https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2016.23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sniderman, P. M., Brody, R. A., & Tetlock, P. E. (1991). Reasoning and choice: Explorations in political psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sniderman, P. M., & Stiglitz, E. H. (2012). The reputational premium. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science,50, 755–769.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank participants in the “New Developments in the Study of Political Persuasion” conference at UC Irvine for valuable feedback. Thank you as well to the anonymous reviewers and the Editor for their excellent suggestions.

Funding

This research was generously funded by an Interdisciplinary Research Grant from the University of California, Davis. We are grateful to Danielle Joesten Martin for outstanding research assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cheryl Boudreau.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

All procedures involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in this study.

Additional information

Data and replication files are available on Dataverse (https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/U6YTI5).

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 1189 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Boudreau, C., Elmendorf, C.S. & MacKenzie, S.A. Roadmaps to Representation: An Experimental Study of How Voter Education Tools Affect Citizen Decision Making. Polit Behav 41, 1001–1024 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9480-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-9480-6

Keywords

Navigation