Skip to main content
Log in

On the grammar of extraction and coordination

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper examines English unbounded dependency constructions and concludes that the syntactic mechanism responsible for extraction operates in the same way in all constructions, regardless of the structures being subordinate or coordinate. In other words, this work argues that there is a general and fully uniform mechanism behind the linkage of ‘gaps’ and their ‘fillers’ in the family of constructions that allow such unbounded dependencies. Crucially, this mechanism is devoid of exceptions, and interacts with other aspects outside the grammar of extraction to predict the Coordinate Structure Constraint, ATB extractions, parasitism, and at least certain islands. Although the underlying syntactic mechanism for extraction is argued to work in the same way for all constructions, certain parts of the grammar can locally impose additional conditions on the possible extraction patterns. Along with recent psycholinguistic and neurolinguistic research, it is argued that many apparent exceptions result from independently motivated performance factors, also observed in garden-path sentences and center-embedded constructions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abels, Klaus. 2003. Auxiliary adverb word order revisited. In Proceedings of upenn colloquium on linguistics, 26–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abrusán, Márta. 2008. Contradiction and grammar: the case of weak islands. PhD dissertation, MIT.

  • Alexopoulou, Theodora and Frank Keller. 2007. Locality, cyclicity and resumption: at the interface between the grammar and the human sentence processor. Language 83 (1): 110–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Assmann, Anke, Fabian Heck, Johannes Hein, Stefan Keine, and Gereon Mueller. 2010. Does chain hybridization in Irish support movement-based approaches to long-distance dependencies. In Proceedings of the 17th international conference on head-driven phrase structure grammar, University of Parise-Sorbonne, France. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bach, Emmon. 1986. The algebra of events. Linguistics and Philosophy 9: 5–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltin, Mark. 1982. A landing site theory of movement rules. Linguistic Inquiry 13 (1).

  • Beavers, John, and Ivan A. Sag. 2004. Ellipsis and apparent non-constituent coordination. In Müller, Stefan (ed.). Proceedings of the 11th international conference on head-driven phrase structure grammar, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, 48–69. Stanford: CSLI Publications. http://cslipublications.stanford.edu/HPSG/5/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloomfield, Leonard. 1933. Language. New York: Henry Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borgonovo, Claudia, and Ad Neeleman. 2000. Transparent adjuncts. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 45: 199–224. Revue canadienne de linguistique.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borsley, Robert D. 2004. An approach to English comparative correlatives. In Müller, Stefan (ed.). Proceedings of the 11th international conference on head-driven phrase structure grammar, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, 70–925. Stanford: CSLI Publications. http://cslipublications.stanford.edu/HPSG/5/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borsley, Robert D. 2005. Against ConjP. Lingua 4 (115): 461–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouma, Gosse, Rob Malouf, and Ivan A. Sag. 2001. Satisfying constraints on extraction and adjunction. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 1 (19): 1–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chao, Wyn. 1988. On ellipsis. New York: Garland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaves, Rui P. 2008. Linearization-based word-part ellipsis. Linguistics and Philosophy 31 (3): 261–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaves, Rui P. 2009. Construction-based cumulation and adjunct extraction. In Proceedings of the 16th international conference on head-driven phrase structure grammar, University of Göttingen, Germany, 47–67. Stanford: CSLI Publications. http://cslipublications.stanford.edu/HPSG/2009/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaves, Rui P. to appear. Conjunction, cumulation and respectively readings. Journal of Linguistics.

  • Chaves, Rui P., and Ivan A. Sag. 2007. Two kinds of ellipsis in English coordinate structures. Unpublished MS. University at Buffalo and Stanford University.

  • Chen, Evan, Edward Gibson, and Florian Wolf. 2005. Online syntactic storage costs in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 52: 144–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1982. Concepts and consequences of the theory of government and binding. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, Sandra. 1998. The design of agreement: evidence from Chamorro. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, Sandra, and James McCloskey. 1983. On the interpretation of certain island facts in GPSG. Linguistic Inquiry 14: 703–714.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cinque, Gugliemo. 1990. Types of \(\overline{A}\) -dependencies. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clements, George N. 1984. Binding domains in Kikuyu. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 14: 37–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, Jonathan L. 1971. The logical particles of natural language. In Pragmatics of natural language, ed. Yehoshua Bar-Hillel, 50–68. Dordrecht: Reidel.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, Chris. 1994. Economy of derivation and the generalized proper binding condition. Linguistic Inquiry 25: 45–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Copestake, Ann, Alex Lascarides, and Dan Flickinger. 2001. An algebra for semantic construction in constraint-based grammars. In Proc. of the 39th ACL. Toulouse, France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Copestake, Ann, Daniel Flickinger, Ivan A. Sag, and Carl Pollard. 2006. Minimal recursion semantics. Journal Research on Language & Computation 3 (4): 281–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowart, Wayne. 1997. Experimental syntax: applying objective methods to sentence judgments. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crain, Stephen, and Mark Steedman. 1985. On not being led up the garden path: the use of context by the psychological parser. In Natural language parsing: psychological, computational and theoretical perspectives, ACL studies in natural language processing, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, Dowty, David, Lauri Kartunnen and Arnold Zwicky (eds.) 320–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Culicover, Peter W., and Robert D. Levine. 2001. A reconsideration of locative inversion. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 19: 283–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deane, Paul D. 1991. Limits to attention: a cognitive theory of island phenomena. Cognitive Linguistics 2 (1): 1–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deane, Paul D. 1992. Grammar in mind and brain. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Diesing, Molly. 1990. Verb movement and the subject position in Yiddish. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 8: 41–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donohue, Mark, and Anna Maclachlan. 1999. What agreement in Chamorro? Austroesian Formal Linguistics Association 6: 121–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckardt, Regina. 1995. Does information flow in event structures? In Ellipsis, underspecication, events and more in dynamic semantics, dyana-2 deliverable r2.2.c, ed. Geroen Groenendijk, 49–72. ILLC/Department of Philosophy, University of Amsterdam.

  • Ellis, Rod. 1991. Grammaticality judgments and second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 13 (2): 161–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engdahl, Elisabet. 1983. Parasitic gaps. Linguistics and Philosophy 6: 3–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erteschik-Shir, Nomi. 1973. On the nature of island constraints. Doctoral dissertation, Cambridge, MIT.

  • Erteschik-Shir, Nomi. 1981. More on extractability from quasi-nps. Linguistic Inquiry 12: 665–670.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fanselow, Gisbert, and Stefan Frisch. 2004. Effects of processing difficulty on judgments of acceptability. In Gradience in grammar, eds. G. Fanselow, C. Fery, M. Schlesewsky, and R. Vogel. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiengo, Robert, and James Higginbotham. 1981. Opacity in NP. Linguistic Analysis 7 (4): 395–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, Janet. 1978. Parsing strategies and constraints on transformations. Linguistic Inquiry 9: 427–473.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, Janet D. 1983. Phrase structure parsing and the island constraints. Linguistics and Philosophy 6: 163–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, Janet D. 1992. Islands, learnability and the lexicon. In Island constraints: theory, acquisition and processing, eds. Helen Good Luck and Michael Rochemont. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, Lynn. 1987. Syntactic processing: evidence from dutch. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 5: 519–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawron, Jean M., and Andrew Kehler. 2003. Respective answers to coordinated questions. In Semantics and linguistic theory 13 (salt 13). Seattle: University of Washington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazdar, Gerald. 1981. Unbounded dependencies and coordinate structure. Linguistic Inquiry 12 (2): 155–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazdar, Gerald, Ewann Klein, Geoffrey K. Pullum, and Ivan A. Sag. 1985. Generalized phrase structure grammar. Oxford/Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginzburg, Jonathan, and Ivan A. Sag. 2000. Interrogative investigations: the form, meaning and use of English interrogative constructions. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, John. 1985. A principled exception to the coordinate structure constraint. In Papers from the twenty-first annual regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, 133–143. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodall, Grant. 1987. Parallel structures in syntax: coordination, causatives, and restructuring. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosu, Alexander. 1973. On the nonunitary nature of the coordinate structure constraint. Linguistic Inquiry 4: 88–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hankamer, Jorge. 1974. Why there are two than’s in English. In Papers from the ninth regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, 179–191. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hankamer, Jorge, and Ivan A. Sag. 1976. Deep and surface anaphora. Linguistic Inquiry 7 (3): 391–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, Michael. 1990. On adjunct extraction from complements. In Mit working papers in linguistics 13, eds. Lisa Cheng and Hamida Demirdash, 101–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendriks, Petra. 1995. Comparatives and categorial grammar. PhD dissertation, University of Groningen.

  • Hofmeister, Philip. 2007a. Facilitating memory retrieval in natural language comprehension. PhD thesis, Stanford University.

  • Hofmeister, Philip. 2007b. Retrievability and gradience in filler-gap dependencies. In Proceedings from the annual meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society, eds. Malcom Elliott, James Kirby, Osamu Sawada, Eleni Staraki, and Suwon Yoon, Vol. 43(1), 109–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofmeister, Philip, and Ivan A. Sag. 2010. Cognitive constraints and island effects. Language 86 (2): 366–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huddleston, Rodney D., Geoffrey Pullum, and Peter Peterson. 2002. In The Cambridge grammar of the English language, 1033–1096. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hukari, Tom, and Robert D. Levine. 1995. Adjunct extraction. Journal of Linguistics 31: 195–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johannessen, Janne B. 1998. Coordination. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kathol, Andreas. 1993. Linearization and coordination in German. In Papers in syntax, osu working papers in linguistics, vol. 42, eds. Andreas Kathol and Carl J. Pollard, 109–123. Ohio State University, Department of Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kathol, Andreas. 1995. Linearization-based German syntax. PhD Thesis, Ohio State University.

  • Kathol, Andreas. 2000. Linear syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard. 1980. Extensions of binding and case-marking. Linguistic Inquiry 11:75–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard. 1994. The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard, and Jean-Yves Pollock. 1978. Stylistic inversion, successive cyclicity, and Move NP in French. Linguistic Inquiry 9: 93–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehler, Andrew. 2002. Coherence, reference, and the theory of grammar. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Jong-Bok, and Ivan A. Sag. 2005. English object extraposition: a constraint-based approach. In Proceedings of the 12th international conference on head-driven phrase structure grammar, Department of Informatics, University of Lisbon, ed. Stefan Müller, 192–212. Stanford: CSLI Publications. http://cslipublications.stanford.edu/HPSG/6/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kluender, Robert. 1992. Deriving island constraints from principles of predication. In Island constraints: theory, acquisition and processing, eds. Helen Goodluck and Michael Rochemont, 223–258. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kluender, Robert. 1998. On the distinction between strong islands and weak islands: a processing perspective. In Syntax and semantics 29: The limits of syntax, eds. P. W. Culicover and L. McNally, 241–279. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kluender, Robert. 2004. Are subject islands subject to a processing account. In Proceedings of the WCCFL 23, eds. B. Schmeiser, V. Chand, A. Kelleher, and A. Rodriguez, 101–125. Somerville: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kluender, Robert, and Marta Kutas. 1993. Subjacency as a processing phenomenon. Language and Cognitive Processes 8: 573–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konieczny, Lars. 1996. Human sentence processing: a semantics-oriented parsing approach. PhD diss, Universität Freiburg.

  • Kroch, Anthony. 1989. Amount quantification, referentiality, and long wh-movement. Ms., Department of Linguistics, University of Pennsylvania.

  • Kuno, Susumu. 1976. Subject, theme, and the speaker’s empathy—a reexamination of relativization phenomena. In Subject and topic, ed. Charles N. Li, 417–444. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuno, Susumu. 1987. Functional syntax—anaphora, discourse and empathy. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, George. 1986. Frame semantic control of the coordinate structure constraint. In Papers from the 22nd regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lasersohn, Peter. 1995. Plurality, conjunction and events. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, Nancy, and Ellen Prince. 1986. Gapping and clausal implicature. Papers in Linguistics 19: 351–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, Robert, Thomas E. Hukari, and Michael Calcagno. 2001. Parasitic gaps in English: some overlooked cases and their theoretical implications. In Parasitic gaps, eds. Peter Culicover and Paul M. Postal, 181–222. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, Robert D. 2001. The extraction riddle: just what are we missing? Journal of Linguistics 37: 145–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, Robert D. 2003. Adjunct valents: cumulative scoping adverbial constructions and impossible descriptions. In The proceedings of the 9th international conference on head-driven phrase structure grammar, eds. Jongbok Kim and Stephen Wechsler, 209–232. Stanford: CSLI Publications. http://cslipublications.stanford.edu/HPSG/3/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, Robert D., and Thomas E. Hukari. 2006. The unity of unbounded dependency constructions. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, Robert D., and Ivan A. Sag. 2003. Some empirical issues in the grammar of extraction. In Proceedings of the HPSG-2003 conference, Michigan State University, East Lansing, ed. Stefan Müller, 236–256. Stanford: CSLI Publications. http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/HPSG/3/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, Stephen C. 2000. Presumptive meanings: the theory of generalized conversational implicature. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Link, Godehard. 1984. Hydras: on the logic of relative clause constructions with multiple heads. In Varieties of formal semantics, eds. Fred Landman and Frank Veltman, 245–257. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Link, Godehard. 1998. Algebraic semantics in language and philosophy. CSLI lecture notes, vol. 74.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, Maryellen C., Neal J. Pearlmutter, and Mark S. Seidenberg. 1994. The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review 101 (4): 676–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maling, Joan, and Annie Zaenen. 1978. The non-universality of a surface filter. Linguistic Inquiry 9: 475–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maling, Joan, and Annie Zaenen. 1982. A phrase structure account of Scandinavian extraction phenomena. In Jacobson, Pauline & Goeffrey K. Pullum (eds.), The nature of syntactic representation, 229–282. Dordrecht: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCawley, James D. 1981. The syntax and semantics of English relative clauses. Lingua 53: 99–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, James. 1979. Model-theoretic semantics and transformational grammar. Dordrecht: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munn, Alan B. 1993. Topics in the syntax and semantics of coordinate structures. PhD dissertation, University of Maryland, Maryland.

  • Munn, Alan B. 1998. ATB movement without identity. In Proceedings of the 14th eastern states conference on linguistics (ESCOL-97), eds. Jennifer Austin and Aaron Lawson, 150–160. Yale University, CSC Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munn, Alan B. 1999. On the identity requirement of ATB extraction. Natural Language Semantics 7: 421–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oehrle, Richard T. 1987. Boolean properties in the analysis of gapping. In Syntax and semantics 20: discontinuous constituency, eds. Geoffrey J. Huck and Almerindo E. Ojeda, 201–240. Orlando: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oshima, David Y. 2007. On factive islands: pragmatic anomaly vs. pragmatic infelicity. In Proceedings of the 20th annual conference on new frontiers in artificial intelligence. JSAI’06, 147–161. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearlmutter, Neal J., and Maryellen C. MacDonald. 1979. Plausibility and syntactic ambiguity resolution. In Proceedings of the 14th annual conference of the cognitive science society, 498–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pesetsky, David. 1982. Paths and categories. PhD dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.

  • Pesetsky, David. 1987. Wh-in-situ: movement and unselective binding. In The representation of (in)definiteness, eds. Eric Reuland and Alice ter Meulen, 98–129. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pesetsky, David. 2000. Phrasal movement and its kin. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, Martin, and Guy Barry. 1991. Sentence processing without empty categories. Language and Cognitive Processes 6: 229–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollard, Carl, and Ivan A. Sag. 1994. Head-driven phrase structure grammar. Chicago/Stanford: University of Chicago Press/CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postal, Paul M. 1993. Parasitic gaps and the across-the-board phenomenon. Linguistic Inquiry 24: 735–754.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postal, Paul M. 1994. Parasitic and pseudoparasitic gaps. Linguistic Inquiry 25: 63–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postal, Paul M. 1998. Three investigations of extraction. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postal, Paul M. 2001. Islands. In The handbook of syntactic theory, eds. M. Baltin and C. Collins. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, Arnold, and Ray Gary Peters. 1973. Semantic clues in comprehension of novel sentences. Language and Cognitive Processes 6: 229–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pritchett, Bradley L. 1991. Subjacency in a principle-based parser. In Principle-based parsing: computation and psycholinguistics, ed. Robert C. Berwick, 301–345. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pustejovsky, James. 1995. The generative lexicon. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, John. 1967. Constraints on variables in syntax. Doctoral dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, Massachusetts. [Published in 1986 as Infinite Syntax! Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing].

  • Sag, Ivan A. 2000. Another argument against wh-trace. Jorge Hankamer Webfest. http://ling.ucsc.edu/Jorge/sag.html.

  • Sag, Ivan A. 2002. Coordination and underspecification. In Proceedings of the 9th international conference on head-driven phrase structure grammar, Seoul: Kyung Hee University, 267–2913.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sag, Ivan A. 2005. Adverb extraction and coordination: a reply to Levine. In The proceedings of the 12th international conference on head-driven phrase structure grammar, Department of Informatics, University of Lisbon, ed. Stefan Müller, 322–342. Stanford: CSLI Publications. http://cslipublications.stanford.edu/HPSG/6/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sag, Ivan A. 2010a. English filler-gap constructions. Language 86 (3): 486–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sag, Ivan A. 2010b. Sign-based construction grammar: an informal synopsis. to appear in H. Boas and I. A. Sag (eds.), Sign-based construction grammar. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

  • Sag, Ivan A., and Janet Fodor. 1994. Extraction without traces. In Proceedings of the thirteenth West Coast conference on formal linguistics, 365–384. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sag, Ivan A., and Thomas Wasow. 2011. Performance-compatible competence grammar. In Non-transformational syntax, eds. Robert Borsley and Kersti Borjars. Cambridge: Blackwells.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sag, Ivan A., Philip Hofmeister, and Neal Snider. 2007. Processing complexity in subjacency violations: the complex noun phrase constraint. In Proceedings of the 43rd annual meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. Chicago: CLS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sag, Ivan A., Thomas Wasow, and Emily M. Bender. 2003. Syntactic theory—a formal introduction. 2nd edn. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sag, Ivan A., Geral Gazdar, Thomas Wasow, and Steven Weisler. 1985. Coordination and how to distinguish categories. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 3: 117–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sato, Yo, and Wai Lok Tam. 2008. Towards a unified account of adjuncts. In The proceedings of the 15th international conference on head-driven phrase structure grammar, ed. Stefan Müller, 173–188. Stanford: CSLI Publications. http://cslipublications.stanford.edu/HPSG/9/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmerling, Susan. 1972. Apparent counterexamples to the coordinate structure constraint: a canonical constraint. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 2 (1): 91–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steedman, Mark. 1989. Constituency and coordination in combinatory grammar. In Alternative conceptions of phrase structure, eds. Mark Baltin and Anthony Kroch. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stolz, Walter S. 1976. A study of the ability to decode grammatically novel sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 6: 867–873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stowe, Lauriea. 1986. Parsing wh-constructions: evidence for on-line gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes 1: 227–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Eynde, Frank. 2003. On the notion ‘determiner’. In Proceedings of the HPSG-2003 conference, Michigan State University, East Lansing, ed. Stefan Müller, 391–396. Stanford: CSLI Publications. http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/HPSG/3/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vasishth, Shravan, and Richard L. Lewis. 2006. Argument-head distance and processing complexity: explaining both locality and antilocality effects. Language 82 (4): 767–794.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, Tessa, and Edward Gibson. 2002. The influence of referential processing on sentence complexity. Cognition 85: 79–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, Tessa, and Edward Gibson. 2005. Effects of np type and in reading cleft sentences in English. Language and Cognitive Processes 20: 751–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, Edwin. 1978. Across-the-board rule application. Linguistic Inquiry 19: 31–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, Deirdre M. 1975. Presupposition and non-truth-conditional semantics. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yngve, Victor. 1960. A model and an hypothesis for language structure. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 104: 444–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwart, Jan-Wouter. 2005. Some notes on coordination in head-final languages. In Linguistics in the Netherlands 2005, eds. Jenny Doetjes and Jeroen van de Weijer, 232–241. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rui P. Chaves.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chaves, R.P. On the grammar of extraction and coordination. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 30, 465–512 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-011-9164-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-011-9164-y

Keywords

Navigation