Abstract
Identifying causes of healthy behaviors is important for harnessing health benefits. A growing body of experience sampling research suggests that positive emotion may drive these behaviors. However, it is not known how long elevations in positive emotion facilitate these behaviors in daily life. The present study tested how time between signals moderates the association between within-person positive affect and healthy behaviors. A sample of 197 college students completed a 10-day experience sampling diary, with 5 signals a day, measuring affect and healthy behaviors. We replicated results from Nylocks and colleagues (2018) finding that within-person positive affect predicted engagement in healthy behaviors; however, this association was only significant within the same day, and not across days (i.e. overnight). Taken together, these findings highlight the importance of considering positive affect, rather than negative affect in patients with psychopathology, to improve behavioral interventions targeted to increase engagement in healthy behaviors.



Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles and news from researchers in related subjects, suggested using machine learning.Notes
Participants in the healthy control group were determined eligible if they met the following inclusion criteria: 1) Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF; rated from the SCID-I) score was greater than 79, 2) participants had an absence of any Axis-I pathology (full or partially remitted) in the past 12 months, 3) no current symptoms of any depressive or anxiety disorder, 4) absence of any personality disorder (i.e. less than two symptoms endorsed on any SCID-II scale), 5) no use of psychiatric medications in the past 12 months, and 6) no evidence of elevated social desirability (i.e. scores greater than 25 on the Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale; Crowne and Marlowe, 1960).
Because participants could delay responding to signals for up to 2 h, some participants were able to respond to signal t during the time interval they received the prompt for signal t + 1. For example, if signal t was received at 11 am, and delayed for 2 h signal t could be completed by 1 pm, however, signal t + 1 could have been received at 12:00 pm. This then led to instances where participants responded to signals right after one another leaving very little time between signal responses. For example, a participant who received the prompt for signal t at 10:30 am received the prompt for signal t + 1 at 11:30 am and responded to both signals back to back at 12:00 pm and 12:01 pm.
A sensitivity analysis was conducted to include response rate as a covariate, due to lower than normal compliance rate. Total number of diary responses was included as a covariate. Results remained consistent with the original analysis (see Table S2 in the supplemental materials for more detail).
Sensitivity analyses were conducted to examine the associations of PA and NA separately. The main effect associated with Aim 1 and the moderation effects associated with Aim 2 remained consistent for each model compared to the original models (See tables S3-S5 for more detailed information.
Additional analysis were conducted to examine the reciprocal relationship between healthy behaviors (t) and positive affect (t + 1). The main effect of healthy behaviors (analogous to Aim 1) was not statistically significant, however moderation effects of time (analogous to Aim 2) were statistically significant such that as the time between signals t and t + 1 decreased, the effect of healthy behaviors (t) and positive affect (t + 1) increased (See tables S6-S8 for more detailed information).
References
Aurora, P., & Coifman, K. G. (2021). Unpacking social avoidance and substance use in social anxiety: Does extraversion explain behavior variability? Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 43(2), 281–292.
Barrett, L. F., Lewis, M., & Haviland-Jones, J. (2016). Handbook of Emotions (4th ed.). The Guilford Press.
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistics Society, 57, 289–290.
Berger, B. G., & Motl, R. W. (2000). Exercise and mood: A selective review and synthesis of research employing the profile of mood states. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 12, 69–92.
Berkhof, J., & Snijders, T. A. (2001). Variance component testing in multilevel models. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 26(2), 133–152.
Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review Psychology, 54, 579–616.
Burgdorf, J., & Panksepp, J. (2006). The neurobiology of positive emotions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 173–187.
Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J. P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2014). Toward a neurology of loneliness. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 1464–1504. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037618
Cameron, D. S., Bertenshaw, E. J., & Sheeran, P. (2018). Positive affect and physical activity: Testing effects on goal setting, activation, prioritisation, and attainment. Psychology & Health, 33(2), 258–274. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2017.1314477
Cardi, V., Leppanen, J., Leslie, M., Esposito, M., & Treasure, J. (2019). The use of a positive mood induction video-clip to target eating behaviour in people with bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder: An experimental study. Appetite, 133, 400–404. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2018.12.001
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibtion, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 319–333.
Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2008). Positive psychological well-being and mortality: A quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70(7), 741–756.
Chue, A. E., Gunthert, K. C., Kim, R. W., Alfano, C. A., & Ruggiero, A. R. (2018). The role of sleep in adolescents’ daily stress recovery: Negative affect spillover and positive affect bounce-back effects. Journal of Adolescent Health, 66, 101–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.05.006
Cohen, S., Alper, C. M., Doyle, W. J., & Treanor, J. J. (2006). Positive emotional style predicts resistance to illness after experimental exposure to rhinovirus or influenza A virus. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 809–815.
Cohn, M. A., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). In search of durable positive psychology interventions: Predictors and consequences of long-term positive behavior change. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(5), 355–366. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2010.508883
Mehl & Conner (2012). Handbook of research methods for studying daily life. Guilford Press.
Cooper, M. L., Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Munder, P. (1995). Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(5), 990–1005.
Corr, P. J. (2008). The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511819384
Cranford, J. A., Shrout, P. E., Iida, M., Rafaeli, E., Yip, T., & Bolger, N. (2006). A procedure for evaluating sensitivity to within-person change: Can mood measures in diary studies detect change reliably? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(7), 917–929.
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24(4), 349.
Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., et al. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 564–570.
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6(3–4), 169–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939208411068
Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach-avoidance motivation in personality: Approach and avoidance temperaments and goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(5), 804–818. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.5.804
Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open hearts build lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1045–1062.
Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crises? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 365–376.
Fredrickson, B. L., Arizmendi, C., & Van Cappellen, P. (in press). Same-day, cross-day, and upward spiral relations between positive affect and positive health behaviours. Psychology & Health.
Gilman, T. L., Shaheen, R., Nylocks, N. M., Halachoff, D., Chapman, J., Flynn, J. J., Matt, L. M., & Coifman, K. G. (2017). A film set for the elicitation of emotion in research: A comprehensive catalog derived from four decades of investigation. Behavioral Research Methods, 49(6), 2061–2082.
Gray, J. A. (1994). Personality dimensions and emotion systems. The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 329–331). Oxford University Press.
Griskevicius, V., Shiota, M. N., & Neufeld, S. L. (2010). Influence of different positive emotions on persuasion processing: A functional evolutionary approach. Emotion, 10(2), 190–206.
Grosz, M. P., Rohrer, J. M., & Thoemmes, F. (in press). The taboo against explicit causal inference in nonexperimental psychology. Perspectives in Psychological Science.
Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Peterson, C. K. (2010). The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: A review and update. Biological Psychology, 84, 451–462. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.08.010
Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110(1), 86–108.
Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Forster, G., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Everyday Temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 1318–1335.
Hogan, C. L., Mata, J., & Carstensen, L. L. (2013). Exercise holds immediate benefits for affect and cognition in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 28(2), 587–594.
Holt-Lunstad, J., & Smith, T. B. (2012). Social relationships and mortality. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6(1), 41–53.
Kanning, M. K., & Schoebi, D. (2016). Momentary affective states are associated with momentary volume, prospective trends, and fluctuation of daily physical activity. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 744.
Kim, E. S., Kubzansky, L. D., Soo, J., & Boehm, J. K. (2017). Maintaining healthy behavior: A prospective study of psychological well-being and physical activity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine: A Publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, 51(3), 337–346.
Konjarski, M., Murray, G., Lee, V. V., & Jackson, M. L. (2018). Reciprocal relationships between daily sleep and mood: A systematic review of naturalistic prospective studies. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 42, 47–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2018.05.005
Lejuez, C. W., Hopko, D. R., & Hopko, S. D. (2001). A brief behavioral activation treatment for depression. Treatment Manual. Behavioral Modification, 25(2), 255–286. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445501252005
Loewenstein, G., & Lerner, J. S. (2003). The role of affect in decision making. In R. Davidson, H. Goldsmith, & K. Scherer (Eds.), Handbook of Affective Science (pp. 619–642). Oxford.
Maher, J. P., Doerkson, S. E., Elavsky, S., Hyde, A. L., Pincus, A. L., Ram, N., & Conroy, D. E. (2013). A daily analysis of physical activity and satisfaction with life in emerging adults. Health Psychology, 32, 647–656.
Mata, J., Thompson, R. J., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2012). Walk on the bright side: Physical activity and affect in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(2), 297–308. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023533
Minkel, J. D., Banks, S., Htaik, O., Moreta, M. C., Jones, C. W., McGlinchey, E. L., & Dinges, D. F. (2012). Sleep deprivation and stressors: Evidence for elevated negative affect in response to mild stressors when sleep deprived. Emotion, 12(5), 1015–1020.
Mochon, D., Norton, M. I., & Ariely, D. (2008). Getting off the hedonic treadmill, one step at a time: The impact of regular religious practice and exercise on well-being. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29(5), 632–642.
Murray, G., Allen, N. B., & Trinder, J. (2002). Mood and the circadian system: Investigation of a circadian component in positive affect. Chronobiology International, 19(6), 1151–1169. https://doi.org/10.1081/CBI-120015956
Murray, G., Nicholas, C. L., Kleiman, J., Dwyer, R., Carrington, M. J., Allen, N. B., & Trinder, J. (2009). Nature’s clocks and human mood: The circadian system modulates reward motivation. Emotion, 9(5), 705–716. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017080
Nylocks, K. M., Rafaeli, E., Bar-Kalifa, E., Flynn, J. J., & Coifman, K. G. (2018). Testing the influence of negative and positive emotion on future health-promoting behaviors in a community sample. Motivation and Emotion, 43, 285–298. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-018-9729-8
Ong, A. D. (2010). Pathways linking positive emotion and health in later life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 358–362. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721410388805
Ong, A. D., Kim, S., Young, S., & Steptoe, A. (2017). Positive affect and sleep: A systematic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 35, 21–32.
Ottenstein, C., & Werner, L. (in press). Compliance in Ambulatory Assessment Studies: Investigating Study and Sample Characteristics as Predictors. Assessment.
Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interaction effects in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437–448.
Quoidbach, J., Taquet, M., Desseilles, M., de Montjoye, Y. A., & Gross, J. J. (2019). Happiness and social behavior. Psychological Science, 30(8), 1111–1122.
Rhodes, R. E., Fiala, B., & Conner, M. (2009). A review and meta-analysis of affective judgments and physical activity in adult populations. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 38(3), 180–204. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-009-9147-y
Rhodes, R. E., & Kates, A. (2015). Can the affective response to exercise predict future motives and physical activity behavior? a systematic review of published evidence. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 49(5), 715–731. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-015-9704-5
Rice, E. L., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2017a). Do spontaneous thoughts function as incentive salience? Emotion, 17, 840–855. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000284
Rice, E. L., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2017b). Of passions and positive spontaneous thoughts. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41, 350–361. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-016-9755-3
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161–1178.
Salovey, P., Rothman, A. J., Detweiler, J. B., & Steward, W. T. (2000). Emotional states and physical health. American Psychologist, 55, 110–121.
Schöndube, A., Kanning, M., & Fuchs, R. (2016). The bidirectional effect between momentary affective states and exercise duration on a day level. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1414.
Selby, E. A., Kranzler, A., Panza, E., & Fehling, K. B. (2016). Bidirectional-compounding effects of rumination and negative emotion in predicting impulsive behavior: Implications for emotional cascades. Journal of Personality, 84(2), 139–153.
Selig, J. P., Preacher, K. J., & Little, T. D. (2012). Modeling time-dependent association in longitudinal data: A lag as moderator approach. Multivariate Behavior Research, 47(5), 697–716.
Seligman, M. E. P., Steen, T. A., Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2005). Positive psychology progress: Empirical validation of interventions. American Psychologist, 60, 410–421.
Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention—behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. European Review of Social Psychology, 12(1), 1–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792772143000003
Sheeran, P., & Webb, T. L. (2016). The intention-behavior gap. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10, 503–518. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12265
Shrout, P. E., & Lane, S. P. (2012). Psychometrics. In M. R. Mehl & T. S. Conner (Eds.), Handbook of research methods for studying daily life (pp. 302–320). Guilford Press.
Shrout, P. E., Stadler, G., Lane, S. P., McClure, M. J., Jackson, G. L., Clavél, F. D., & Bolger, N. (2018). Initial elevation bias in subjective reports. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(1), E15–E23.
Snijders, T. A. B., & Bosker, R. J. (2012). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling – (2nd ed.). SAGE Publications.
Snir, A., Rafaeli, E., Gadassi, R., Berenson, K., & Downey, G. (2015). Explicit and inferred motives for nonsuicidal self-injurious acts and urges in borderline and avoidant personality disorders. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 6(3), 267–277. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000104
Sonnentag, S. (2001). Work, recovery activities, and individual wellbeing: A diary study. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 6, 196–210.
Steel, P., Schmidt, J., & Shultz, J. (2008). Refining the relationship between personality and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 138–161. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138
Taquet, M., Quoidbach, J., de Montjoye, Y., Desseilles, M., & Gross, J. J. (2016). Hedonism and the choice of everyday activities. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(35), 9769–9773. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1519998113
Thayer, R. E., Newman, J. R., & McClain, T. M. (1994). Self-regulation of mood: Strategies for changing a bad mood, raising energy, and reducing tension. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(5), 910–925.
Tice, D. M., Bratsklavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 53–67.
Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(2), 320–333. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.320
Updegraff, J. A., Gable, S. L., & Taylor, S. E. (2004). What makes experiences satisfying? The interaction of approach-avoidance motivation and emotions in well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 496–504.
Widdershoven, R. L., Wichers, M., Kuppens, P., Hartmann, J. A., Menne-Lothmann, C., Simons, C. J., & Bastiaansen, J. A. (2019). Effect of self-monitoring through experience sampling on emotion differentiation in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 244, 71–77.
Zautra, A. J., Reich, J. W., Davis, M. C., Potter, P. T., & Nicolson, N. A. (2000). The role of stressful events in the relationship between positive and negative affects: Evidence from field and experimental studies. Journal of Personality, 68(5), 927–951.
Zawadzki, M. J., Smyth, J. M., & Costigan, H. J. (2015). Real-time associations between engaging in leisure and daily health and well-being. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 49(4), 605–615.
Zhang, J., & Zheng, Y. (2017). How do academic stress and leisure activities influence college students’ emotional well-being? A daily diary investigation. Journal of Adolescence, 60, 114–121.
Funding
No funding was received for conducting this study.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.
Research involving human and animal participants
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study was approved by the Kent State University IRB 17-029.
Informed consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Aurora, P., Disabato, D.J. & Coifman, K.G. Positive affect predicts engagement in healthy behaviors within a day, but not across days. Motiv Emot 46, 211–225 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-021-09924-z
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-021-09924-z