Skip to main content
Log in

Visual Attention and Lexical Involvement in L1 and L2 Word Processing: Emotional Stroop Effect

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Given the fact that the process of engaging and interacting with the text is not only the function of a reader but also the text itself, the current study attempts to examine the role of the type of the word in the attentional engagement. More specifically, the present investigation aims to verify the interplay of sensorimotor information, emotions, and the linguistic information in the word processing. In so doing, for the scale validation, a sample of 220 Iranian English as a Foreign Language (EFL) learners from different language institutes were requested to complete the newly designed Persian and English lexical involvement scales. The results of Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) supported the factor structure and the reliability of the measures. Moreover, using Emotional Stroop task in the experimental set up, the results revealed that there is a strong positive relationship between lexical involvement and visual attentional engagement in L1(Persian). The association was conversely negative in L2 (English). Finally, the statistical analysis indicates that the lexical stimuli differ regarding their magnitude of lexical involvement in L1 and L2.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Altarriba, J. (2003). Does cariño equal “liking”? A theoretical approach to conceptual nonequivalence between languages. International Journal of Bilingualism, 7(3), 305–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, B. A. (2019). Neurobiology of value-driven attention. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 27–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptions of perceptual symbols. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 637–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bartolozzi, C., & Indiveri, G. (2006). Selective attention implemented with dynamic synapses and integrate-and-fire neurons. Neurocomputing, 69(16–18), 1971–1976.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Rush, J. A., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Preface. In A. T. Beck, J. A. Rush, B. F. Shaw, & G. Emery (Eds.), Cognitive therapy of depression (pp. 6–33). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, S. I., Harris, A. M., Venini, D., & Retell, J. D. (2014). Visual search for color and shape: When is the gaze guided by feature relationships, when by feature values? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(1), 264–291.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bello, P. F., Bridewell, W., & Wasylyshyn, C. (2016). Attentive and pre-attentive processes in multiple object tracking: A computational investigation. In Proceedings of the thirty-eighth annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 1127–1139). Philadelphia, PA.

  • Ben-Haim, M. S., Williams, P., Howard, Z., Mama, Y., Eidels, A., & Algom, D. (2016). The Emotional Stroop Task: Assessing cognitive performance under exposure to emotional content. JoVE (Journal of Visualized Experiments), 112, e53720–e53720.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Yehudah, G., & Brann, A. (2019). Pay attention to digital text: The impact of the media on text comprehension and self-monitoring in higher-education students with ADHD. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 89, 120–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertels, J., Kolinsky, R., & Morais, J. (2010). Emotional valence of spoken words influences the spatial orienting of attention. Acta Psychologica, 134(3), 264–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E., & Gathercole, S. E. (1990). The processing of non-target words: Semantic or not? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 42(1), 3–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 332–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dayan, P., Kakade, S., & Montague, P. R. (2000). Learning and selective attention. Nature Neuroscience, 3(11), 1218–1223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., & Grèzes, J. (2006). The power of simulation: Imagining one's own and other's behavior. Brain Research, 1079(1), 4–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dolgunsöz, E. (2016). Using eye-tracking to measure lexical inferences and its effects on reading rate during EFL reading. Journal of Language and Linguistic Studies, 12(1), 63–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dozois, D. J. A., & Beck, A. T. (2008). Cognitive schemas, beliefs and assumptions. In K. S. Dobson & D. J. A. Dozois (Eds.), Risk factors in depression (pp. 121–143). Oxford, England: Elsevier/Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dozois, D. J. A., & Dobson, K. S. (2001). Information processing and cognitive organization in unipolar depression: Specificity and comorbidity issues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(2), 236–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dudschig, C., de la Vega, I., & Kaup, B. (2014). Embodiment and second-language: Automatic activation of motor responses during processing spatially associated L2 words and emotion L2 words in a vertical Stroop paradigm. Brain and Language, 132, 14–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durđević, D. F., & Jelisavčić, K. (2019). Sensory load and word recognition. Empirical Studies in Psychology, 19–21.

  • Epp, A. M., Dobson, K. S., Dozois, D. J., & Frewen, P. A. (2012). A systematic meta-analysis of the Stroop task in depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(4), 316–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Everaert, T., Spruyt, A., & De Houwer, J. (2013). On the malleability of automatic attentional biases: Effects of feature-specific attention allocation. Cognition & Emotion, 27(3), 385–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferré, P., García, T., Fraga, I., Sánchez-Casas, R., & Molero, M. (2010). Memory for emotional words in bilinguals: Do words have the same emotional intensity in the first and in the second language? Cognition and Emotion, 24(5), 760–785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folk, C. L., Ester, E. F., & Troemel, K. (2009). How to keep attention from straying: Get engaged! Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(1), 127–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A. (1984). Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness and polysemy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 256–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamedi, S. M., Pishghadam, R., & Fadardi, J. S. (2019). The contribution of reading emotions to reading comprehension: the mediating effect of reading engagement using a structural equation modeling approach. Educational Research for Policy and Practice, 1–28.

  • Hayhoe, M. (2000). Vision using routines: A functional account of vision. Visual Cognition, 7, 43–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holt, D. J., Titone, D., Long, L. S., Goff, D. C., Cather, C., Rauch, S. L., et al. (2006). The misattribution of salience in delusional patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 83, 247–256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imai, Y. (2010). Emotions in SLA: New insights from collaborative learning for an EFL classroom. The Modern Language Journal, 94, 278–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juhasz, B. J., & Rayner, K. (2003). Investigating the effects of a set of intercorrelated variables on eye fixation durations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(6), 1312–1318.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87(4), 329–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karami, M., Pishghadam, R., & Baghaei, P. (2019). A probe into EFL learners’ emotioncy as a source of test bias: Insights from differential item functioning analysis. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 60, 170–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practices of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kousta, S. T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). The representation of abstract words: Why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(1), 14–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lachter, J., Ruthruff, E., Lien, M. C., & Mccann, R. S. (2008). Is attention needed for word identification? Evidence from the Stroop paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(5), 950–955.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leach, L., & Samuel, A. G. (2007). Lexical configuration and lexical engagement: When adults learn new words. Cognitive Psychology, 55(4), 306–353.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leroy, G., & Kauchak, D. (2014). The effect of word familiarity on actual and perceived text difficulty. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association, 21(1), 169–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima, S. D., & Inhoff, A. W. (1985). Lexical access during eye fixations in reading: Effects of word-initial letter sequence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11(3), 272–285.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindstromberg, S. (2019). Ratings of the emotional valence and arousal of collocations and their constituent words: How can they be useful in L2 vocabulary research? System, 87, 102–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods, 1, 130–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M. (2006). Stroop effect in language. Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics, 161–165.

  • Malloch, S. (2019). Self, selving, and the education of attention. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 65, 101593.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marian, V., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2004). Self-construal and emotion in bicultural bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(2), 190–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, S. L., & Thomas, J. (2011). Emotional intelligence: Examining construct validity using the emotional stroop. International Journal of Business and Social Science, 2(1), 209–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. S. (2011). Culture, emotion, and expression. In D. K. Kenneth (Ed.), Cross cultural psychology: Contemporary themes and perspectives (pp. 331–344). England: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, S. (2011). Language learner self-concept: complexity, continuity and change. System, 39, 335–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mittershiffthaler, S. C. R., Williams, N. D., Walsh, A. J., Cleare, C., Donaldson, C., Scott, J., et al. (2008). Neural basis of the emotional Stroop interference effect in major depression. Psychological Medicine, 38, 247–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montani, V., Chanoine, V., Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2019a). Frequency-tagged visual evoked responses track syllable effects in visual word recognition. Cortex, 121, 60–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montani, V., Chanoine, V., Stoianov, I. P., Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2019b). Steady state visual evoked potentials in reading aloud: Effects of lexicality, frequency and orthographic familiarity. Brain and Language, 192, 1–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Odegaard, B., Wozny, D. R., & Shams, L. (2015). Biases in visual, auditory, and audiovisual perception of space. PLoS Computational Biology, 11(12), e1004649.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pishghadam, R. (2015, October). Emotioncy in language education: From exvolvement to involvement. Paper presented at the 2nd conference of Interdisciplinary Approaches to Language Teaching, Literature, and Translation Studies. Iran, Mashhad.

  • Pishghadam, R. (2016a, May). Emotioncy, extraversion, and anxiety in willingness to communicate in English. Paper presented at the 5th International Conference on Language, Education and Innovation. England: London.

  • Pishghadam, R. (2016b, March). Giving a boost to the working memory: Emotioncy and cognitive load theory. A paper presented in The First National Conference on English Language Teaching, Literature, and Translation. Ghoochan, Iran.

  • Pishghadam, R., & Abbasnejad, H. (2017). Introducing emotioncy as an invisible force controlling causal decisions: A case of attribution theory. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 48(1), 129–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perlovsky, L. (2009). Language and emotions: Emotional Sapir-Whorf hypothesis. Neural Networks, 22, 518–526.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(3), 380–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Qu, Q., Zhang, Q., & Damian, M. F. (2016). Tracking the time course of lexical access in orthographic production: An event-related potential study of word frequency effects in written picture naming. Brain and Language, 159, 118–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saslow, J., & Ascher, A. (2006a). Top Notch 1: English for today’s world. White Plains, NY: Pearson/ Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saslow, J., & Ascher, A. (2006). Top Notch 2: English for today’s world. White Plains, NY: Pearson/ Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, M., & Lilienthal, A. J. (2019). Domain-specific interpretation of eye tracking data: towards a refined use of the eye-mind hypothesis for the field of geometry. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 101(1), 123–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheikh, N. A., & Titone, D. A. (2013). Sensorimotor and linguistic information attenuate emotional word processing benefits: An eye-movement study. Emotion, 13(6), 1107–1121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, T. M., & Altarriba, J. (2008). Emotion words in the mental lexicon: A new look at the emotional Stroop effect. The Mental Lexicon, 3(1), 29–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y. Y. (2017). Mindfulness applications in education. In The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation (pp. 45–56). Palgrave Macmillan, Cham.

  • Vigliocco, G., Kousta, S., Vinson, D., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2013). The representation of abstract words: What matters? Reply to Paivio's (2013) comment on Kousta et al. (2011). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 288–291.

  • Vigliocco, G., Meteyard, L., Andrews, M., & Kousta, S. (2009). Toward a theory of semantic representation. Language and Cognition, 1(2), 219–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, J., De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., & Van Damme, S. (2013). Competing for attentional priority: temporary goals versus threats. Emotion, 13(3), 587–598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, S. J. (2008). Eye movement control during reading: Effects of word frequency and orthographic familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(1), 205–223.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, S., & Liversedge, S. (2004). Orthographic familiarity influences initial eye fixation positions in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16(1–2), 52–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, R. S., & Morris, R. K. (2004). Eye movements, word familiarity, and vocabulary acquisition. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 312–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Reza Pishghadam.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of Ferdowsi University research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Appendices

Appendix A

See Table 5.

Table 5 A sample item from the English lexical involvement scale

Appendix B

See Table 6.

Table 6 A sample item from the Persian lexical involvement scale

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hamedi, S.M., Pishghadam, R. Visual Attention and Lexical Involvement in L1 and L2 Word Processing: Emotional Stroop Effect. J Psycholinguist Res 50, 585–602 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-020-09709-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-020-09709-4

Keywords

Navigation