Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Use of Prosocial Word Enhances the Processing of Language: Frequency Domain Analysis of Human EEG

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Use of prosocial language enhances human cooperation and harmony. Previous research has shown that talking about helping, sharing and giving to others creates positive impression on others, by which individuals and governments gain public approval. So far, the value judgement of approval and disapproval in terms of prosocial or antisocial has not been investigated in the domain of neuroscience of language. Here, the influence of prosocial words towards neural adaptability for greater acceptance is examined using behavioural response mapping with electroencephalography activities of human brain. The prosocial and antisocial words employing correct and incorrect set of sentences in English are presented to participants for performing grammatical judgement task. Our results show that processing of antisocial word requires larger neurocognitive resources as compared to prosocial one, which is corroborated with our behavioural response time suggesting higher response time for antisocial than prosocial words.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adolphs, R., Baron-Cohen, S., & Tranel, D. (2002). Impaired recognition of social emotions following amygdala damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1264–1274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aftanas, L., Varlamov, A., Pavlov, S., Makhnev, V., & Reva, N. (2001). Event-related synchronization and desynchronization during affective processing: emergence of valence-related time-dependent hemispheric asymmetries in theta and upper alpha band. International Journal of Neuroscience, 110(3–4), 197–219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Augustine, J. R. (1996). Circuitry and functional aspects of the insular lobe in primates including humans. Brain Research Reviews, 22(3), 229–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., & Kensinger, E. A. (2010). Context is routinely encoded during emotion perception. Psychological Science, 21(4), 595–599.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., Lindquist, K. A., & Gendron, M. (2007). Language as context for the perception of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 327–332.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Borg, J. S., Hynes, C., Van Horn, J., Grafton, S., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2006). Consequences, action, and intention as factors in moral judgments: An fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(5), 803–817.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, M. A., Mehl, M. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2004). Linguistic markers of psychological change surrounding, September 11, 2001. Psychological Science, 15(10), 687–693.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1972). How to make a language user. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 309–351). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demb, J. B., Desmond, J. E., Wagner, A. D., Vaidya, C. J., Glover, G. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. (1995). Semantic encoding and retrieval in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: A functional MRI study of task difficulty and process specificity. Journal of Neuroscience, 15(9), 5870–5878.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dillon, D. G., Cooper, J. J., Grent, T., Woldorff, M. G., & LaBar, K. S. (2006). Dissociation of event-related potentials indexing arousal and semantic cohesion during emotional word encoding. Brain and Cognition, 62(1), 43–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Hofer, C., Sulik, M. J., & Liew, J. (2014). The development of prosocial moral reasoning and a prosocial orientation in young adulthood: Concurrent and longitudinal correlates. Developmental Psychology, 50(1), 58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Lennon, R., & Roth, K. (1983). Prosocial development: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 19(6), 846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2005). More than talk: Relations between emotion understanding and positive behaviour in toddlers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 343–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estes, Z., & Verges, M. (2008). Freeze or flee? Negative stimuli elicit selective responding. Cognition, 108(2), 557–565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A. D., Hahne, A., & Saddy, D. (2002). Distinct neurophysiological patterns reflecting aspects of syntactic complexity and syntactic repair. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(1), 45–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frimer, J. A., Aquino, K., Gebauer, J. E., Zhu, L. L., & Oakes, H. (2015). A decline in prosocial language helps explain public disapproval of the US Congress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(21), 6591–6594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Girard, L. C., Pingault, J. B., Doyle, O., Falissard, B., & Tremblay, R. E. (2017). Expressive language and prosocial behaviour in early childhood: Longitudinal associations in the UK Millennium Cohort Study. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14(4), 381–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. T., Stern, J. A., Brett, B. E., & Cassidy, J. (2017). The multifaceted nature of prosocial behavior in children: Links with attachment theory and research. Social Development, 24, 661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 155–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haruno, M., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Activity in the amygdala elicited by unfair divisions predicts social value orientation. Nature Neuroscience, 13(2), 160–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jansari, A., Tranel, D., & Adolphs, R. (2000). A valence-specific lateral bias for discriminating emotional facial expressions in free field. Cognition and Emotion, 14(3), 341–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kacewicz, E., Pennebaker, J. W., Davis, M., Jeon, M., & Graesser, A. C. (2014). Pronoun use reflects standings in social hierarchies. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 33(2), 125–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2007). Concreteness in emotional words: ERP evidence from a hemifield study. Brain Research, 1148, 138–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Amygdala activity is associated with the successful encoding of item, but not source, information for positive and negative stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(9), 2564–2570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kissler, J., Herbert, C., Winkler, I., & Junghofer, M. (2009). Emotion and attention in visual word processing: An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 80(1), 75–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knoch, D., & Fehr, E. (2007). Resisting the power of temptations. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1104(1), 123–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krahé, B. (2013). The social psychology of aggression. Milton Park: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuepper, Y., Alexander, N., Osinsky, R., Mueller, E., Schmitz, A., Netter, P., et al. (2010). Aggression: Interactions of serotonin and testosterone in healthy men and women. Behavioural Brain Research, 206(1), 93–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307(5947), 161–163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, B., Declerck, C. H., Emonds, G., & Boone, C. (2017). Trust as commodity: Social value orientation affects the neural substrates of learning to cooperate. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(4), 609–617.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1), 167–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montoya, E. R., Terburg, D., Bos, P. A., & Van Honk, J. (2012). Testosterone, cortisol, and serotonin as key regulators of social aggression: A review and theoretical perspective. Motivation and Emotion, 36(1), 65–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, M. L., Pennebaker, J. W., Berry, D. S., & Richards, J. M. (2003). Lying words: Predicting deception from linguistic styles. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(5), 665–675.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Obsuth, I., Eisner, M. P., Malti, T., & Ribeaud, D. (2015). The developmental relation between aggressive behaviour and prosocial behaviour: A 5-year longitudinal study. BMC psychology, 3(1), 16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Palazova, M., Mantwill, K., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2009). Effects of emotion in words are localized in post-lexical processing stages. In Psychophysiology (Vol. 46, pp. S160–S160). Commerce place, 350 main st., Malden 02148, MA, USA: Wiley-Blackwell Publishing, Inc.

  • Pennebaker, J. W., & King, L. A. (1999). Linguistic styles: Language use as an individual difference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rhee, S. H., Boeldt, D. L., Friedman, N. P., Corley, R. P., Hewitt, J. K., Young, S. E., et al. (2013). The role of language in concern and disregard for others in the first years of life. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Opinion: Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 661.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roehm, D., Schlesewsky, M., Bornkessel, I., Frisch, S., & Haider, H. (2004). Fractionating language comprehension via frequency characteristics of the human EEG. NeuroReport, 15(3), 409–412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogalsky, C., & Hickok, G. (2008). Selective attention to semantic and syntactic features modulates sentence processing networks in anterior temporal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 19(4), 786–796.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300(5626), 1755–1758.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Fabes, R. A., Valiente, C., Shepard, S. A., et al. (2006). Relation of emotion-related regulation to children's social competence: A longitudinal study. Emotion, 6(3), 498–510. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.498.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tabibnia, G., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). The sunny side of fairness: Preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry). Psychological Science, 19(4), 339–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tabullo, Á., Sevilla, Y., Pasqualetti, G., Vernis, S., Segura, E., et al. (2011). Expectancy modulates a late positive ERP in an artificial grammar task. Brain Research, 1373, 131–143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van de Meerendonk, N., Rueschemeyer, S. A., & Kolk, H. H. (2013). Language comprehension interrupted: Both language errors and word degradation activate Broca’s area. Brain and Language, 126(3), 291–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenberghe, R., Nobre, A. C., & Price, C. J. (2002). The response of left temporal cortex to sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(4), 550–560.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Von Hippel, W., & Trivers, R. (2011). Reflections on self-deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(1), 41–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, A. D., Paré-Blagoev, E. J., Clark, J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2001). Recovering meaning: Left prefrontal cortex guides controlled semantic retrieval. Neuron, 31(2), 329–338.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, R. E., Davidson, R. J., & Tomarken, A. J. (1993). Frontal brain asymmetry and emotional reactivity: A biological substrate of affective style. Psychophysiology, 30(1), 82–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, P. B., Poljacik, G., Decety, J., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2017). Loving-kindness language exposure leads to changes in sensitivity to imagined pain. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 2, 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wispe, L. G. (1972). Positive forms of social behavior: An overview. Journal of Social Issues, 28(3), 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Funding was provided by Seed Grant, NIT Raipur (Grant No. NITRR/SeedGrant/2016-17/009), Department of Science and Technology, Government of West Bengal (IN) (Grant No. SERB/ECR/00015).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Arindam Bit.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tarai, S., Mukherjee, R., Qurratul, Q.A. et al. Use of Prosocial Word Enhances the Processing of Language: Frequency Domain Analysis of Human EEG. J Psycholinguist Res 48, 145–161 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-018-9595-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-018-9595-2

Keywords

Navigation