Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Nonverbal Decoding Ability of Children Exposed to Family Violence or Maltreatment: Prospective Evidence from a British Cohort

  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The prospective association between exposure to family violence or maltreatment in the first four years of life and nonverbal decoding ability at age 8.5 years was examined in a British birth cohort. Overall differences were very minor, except it was found that children exposed to maternal victimization were less accurate in decoding low intensity expressions of fear, than were children from nonviolent homes. Children from violent homes were also more likely to identify expressions of anger, fear, and sadness as happiness. Maltreated children showed a global deficit in decoding emotions, but only when presented as high intensity expressions. Whilst these results point to longitudinal associations between these variables, they do not discount the potential impact of third variables. Results are discussed in relation to current theoretical positions, and the need for further research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Balge, K. A., & Milner, J. S. (2000). Emotion recognition ability in mothers at high and low risk for child physical abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 24, 1289–1298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, B., Hanna, E. W., & Parker, G. (1983). Life events for obstetric groups. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 27, 313–320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, K. C., & Campos, J. J. (1987). Perspectives on emotional development II: A functionalist approach to emotions. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 555–578). Oxford: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowen, E., Heron, J., Waylen, A., Wolke, D., & the ALSPAC study team. (2005). Domestic violence risk during and after pregnancy: Evidence from a British longitudinal study. British Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 112, 1083–1089.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyatzis, C. J., & Satyaprasad, C. (1994). Children’s facial and gestural decoding and encoding: Relations between skills and with popularity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 37–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. (1978). Social origins of depression: A study of psychiatric disorder in women. London: Tavistock Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camras, L. A. (1985). Socialization of affect communication. In M. Lewis, & C. Saarni (Eds.), The socialization of emotions (pp. 141–160). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camras, L., Ribordy, S., Hill, J., Martino, S., Sachs, V., Spacarelli, S., & Stefani, R. (1990). Maternal facial behavior and the recognition and production of emotional expression by maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 304–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camras, L., Ribordy, S., Hill, J., Martino, S., Spacarelli, S., & Stefani, R. (1988). Maternal facial behavior and the recognition and production of emotional expression by maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 304–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carton, J. S., Kessler, E. A., & Paper, C. L. (1999). Nonverbal decoding skills and relationship well-being in adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 91–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caspi A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., Taylor, A., & Poulton, R. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, 851–854.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, M. R. (1977). Personality and the structure of the nonverbal communication of emotion. Journal of Personality, 45, 564–584.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Denham, S. A., Zoller, S., & Couchoud, E. A. (1994). Socialization of preschoolers’ emotion understanding. Developmental Psychology, 30, 928–936.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, R., Manstead, A. S., & MacDonald, C. J. (1984). The relationship between children’s sociometric status and ability to recognize facial expressions of emotion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 14, 235–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finch, A. J., Jr., & Childress, W. B. (1975). A comparison of WISC selected subtest short forms with. mentally retarded children. Mental Retardation, 13, 20–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Firth, E. A., Conger, J. C., Kuhlenschmidt, S., & Dorcey, T. (1986). Social competence and social perceptivity. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 4, 85–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R. (1991). Gender and emotion in mother-child conversations about the past. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 4, 325–341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R.(1993). Emotional content of parent-child conversations about the past. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), Memory and affect in development: The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 26, pp. 39–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A., Nwokah, E., Dedo, J. Y., Messinger, P., Dickinson, K. L., Matusou, E., & Holt, S. A. (1992). Social process theory of emotion: A dynamic systems approach. Social Development, 2, 122–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golding, J., Pembrey, M., Jones, R., et al. (2001). ALSPAC – The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. 1. Study methodology. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 15, 74–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halberstadt, A. G. (1983). Family expressiveness styles and nonverbal communication skills. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 8, 14–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halberstadt, A. G. (1986). Family socialization of emotion expression and nonverbal communication styles and skills. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 827–836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herba, C., & Phillips, M. (2004). Annotation: Development of facial expression recognition from childhood to adolescence: Behavioural and neurological perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1185–1199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgins, H. S., & Belch, C. (2000). Interparental violence and nonverbal abilities. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 3–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holtzworth-Munroe, A., Stuart, G. L., Sandin, E., Smultzer, N., & McLaughlin, W. (1997). Comparing the social support behaviors of violent and nonviolent husbands during discussions of wife personal problems. Personal Relationships, 4, 395–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klinnert, M. D., Emde, R. N., Butterfield, P., & Capos, J. J. (1986). Social referencing: The infants use of emotional signals from a friendly adult with mother present. Developmental Psychology, 22, 427–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psychology, 25, 343–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meadows, S. (1996). Parenting behaviour and children’s cognitive development. Hove, England: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClure, E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 424–453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. (2001). Reassessing emotion recognition performance in people with mental retardation: A review. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 106, 481–502.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, C. S., & de Haan, M. (1997). A neurobehavioral approach to the recognition of facial expressions in infancy. In J. A. Russell, & J. M. (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 176–204). New York: Cambridge University Press.

  • Noller, P., & Feeney, J. A. (1994). Relationship satisfaction, attachment and nonverbal accuracy in early marriage. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 199–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., Jr., & Carton, E. (1997). The relation of nonverbal processing ability of faces and voices and children’s feelings of depression and competence. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 158, 357–363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., Jr., & Duke, M. P. (1994). Individual differences in the nonverbal communication of affect: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy Scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 9–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., Jr., & Duke, M. P. (1992). The association of children’s nonverbal decoding abilities with their popularity, locus of control, and academic achievement. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 153, 385–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • ONS (2001). 2001 Census - First results on population for England & Wales. Office for National Statistics.

  • Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Hornung, K., & Reed, A. (2000). Recognizing emotion in faces: Developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Psychology, 36, 679–688.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Philippot, P., & Feldman, R. S. (1990). Age and social competence in preschoolers’ decoding of facial expression. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 43–54.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rust, J. (1996). WOLD Wechsler Objective Language Dimensions Manual. London, UK: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walby, S., & Allen, J. (2004). Domestic violence, sexual assault and stalking: Findings from the British Crime Survey. Home Office Research Study No. 276. London. Home Office.

  • Wechsler, D., Golombok, S., & Rust, J. (1992). WISC-IIIUK Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children -Third Edition UK Manual. Sidcup, UK: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council, the Wellcome Trust, and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. This publication is the work of the authors and Dr Erica Bowen will serve as a guarantor for the contents of this paper. The authors would also like to thank two reviewers for their comments on an earlier draft of the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erica Bowen.

Appendix

Appendix

Maltreatment Index

Evidence of childhood maltreatment during the first four years of life was ascertained using parental reports of mother-child interaction, neglect of child hygiene, excessive changes of caregiver, sexual abuse, parental cruelty to child, and harsh discipline, obtained from questionnaires administered throughout this period.

The quality of mother-child interactions was determined from a series of postal questionnaires administered when the child was 6, 18, 24, 38, and 42 months old. Questions concerned the frequency with which the mother participated in several activities with the child (e.g., singing, playing, etc.). Each item was responded to using a 3-point Likert-type scale (0 = hardly ever, 2 = often) and scale scores were derived from summing items. At each time point, those mothers whose parenting scores approximated the lowest ten percent were identified as exhibiting neglectful parenting. From this, a cumulative neglectful parenting index was developed that indicated how many times across the 5 questionnaires mothers fell into this category. Those who were present in this category at least two times (14%) were labelled neglectful.

Neglect of child hygiene was determined from one question at 6, 24, and 38 months in which mothers were asked how frequently they bathed their child. Those mothers who reported bathing their child either ‘once a week’ or ‘hardly ever’ at each time point were identified as having neglected their child’s hygiene. A cumulative hygiene neglect score was derived from summing the instances in which mothers had been categorized as neglecting the hygiene of their child across the three time points. 1300 (10.6% of the available sample) mothers were categorized as neglecting their children’s hygiene at any of the time points and as a result were coded as ‘hygiene neglecting’ in the maltreatment index.

Mothers were asked at 18, 30, and 42 months whether the child’s primary caregiver had recently changed. In keeping with the methodology employed by Caspi et al. (2002), those children who had experienced 2 or more caregiver changes were integrated into the final index. This related to (3.1%) of the sample.

Mothers were asked at 18, 30, and 42 months whether the child had been sexually abused. Very few mothers reported that their child had been sexually abuse at any of the time points: at 18 months, 5 cases, at 30 months, 4 cases, and at 42 months, 12 cases. Overall, only 21 (0.2%) mothers reported the sexual abuse of a child.

Mothers were asked at 8, 21, 33, and 47 months to report whether they themselves or their partner had been ‘emotionally’ and ‘physically’ cruel to their child. Overall, 970 (8.2%) of children were exposed to some form of parental cruelty within the first 4 years of life.

Fifth, three questions asked of mothers during the relevant period related to the use of harsh discipline. At 18 months, mothers were asked how frequently they smacked their child in order to control temper tantrums. At 24 months, mothers were asked how often they slapped their child, and at 42 months, how often they smacked their child. In order to derive an index of harsh discipline, the number of time points (0–3) in which a child was deemed to have experienced harsh discipline (physical punishment reported frequently at each time point), was calculated. From this, a dichotomous variable was derived based on those mothers who were categorized as having used harsh discipline at any time versus never. This resulted in 1232 cases (10.8%) being categorized as having experienced harsh discipline during the first 4 years of life.

A maltreatment index was derived from summing the number of maltreatment experiences during this 47-month period. In the present study, 4313 (33%) of the cohort had experienced some form of maltreatment. It was found that experiencing some form of maltreatment during the first four years of life increased the likelihood of self-reported participation in some form of antisocial behavior by age 8.5 years by 1.21 times (95% CI: 1.040–1.414, p = .014).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bowen, E., Nowicki, S. The Nonverbal Decoding Ability of Children Exposed to Family Violence or Maltreatment: Prospective Evidence from a British Cohort. J Nonverbal Behav 31, 169–184 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-007-0030-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-007-0030-x

Keywords

Navigation