Skip to main content
Log in

The Perception of Animacy in Young Children with Autism

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Visual perception may be a developmental prerequisite to some types of social understanding. The ability to perceive social information given visual motion appears to develop early. However, children with autism have profound deficits in social cognitive function and may fail to see social motion in the same way that typically developing children do. We tested the hypothesis that children with autism fail to discriminate animate motion, using a novel paradigm involving simple geometric figures. The subjects were 23 children with autism (c.a. 70.7 mos.), 18 children with other developmental disabilities (c.a. 68.2 mos.), and 18 typically developing children (c.a. 46.4 mos.). Children saw two circles moving on a screen and were rewarded for identifying the one that moved as if animate. A control condition required children to identify the heavier of two objects. Children with autism initially showed a deficit in categorizing objects as animate (though no deficit on the control task), but showed no deficit in this ability after they had reached criterion in the training phase. These results are discussed in terms of the social orienting theory of autism, and the possibility that animacy perception might be preserved in autism, even if it is not used automatically.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abell, F., Krams, M., Ashburner, J., Passingham, R., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R., Happe, F., Frith, C., Frith, U. (1999). The neuroanatomy of autism: A voxel-based whole brain analysis of structural scans. Neuroreport, 10(8), 1647–1651.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Joint-attention deficits in autism: Towards a cognitive analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 185–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). The theory of mind deficit in autism: How specific is it? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 301–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Allen, J., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Can autism be detected at 18 months? The needle, the haystack, and the CHAT. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 839–843.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berry, D. S., & Springer, K. (1993). Structure, motion, and preschoolers’ perceptions of social causality. Ecological Psychology, 5, 273–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertenthal, B. I. (1993). Infants’ perception of biomechanical motions: Intrinsic image and knowledge-based constraints. In Visual perception and cognition in infancy (pp. 175–214). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., Spetner, N. B., & Thomas, M. A. (1985). The development of infant sensitivity to biomechanical motions. Child Development, 56, 531–543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14, 151–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buitelaar, J., van Engeland, H., de Kogel, K., de Vries, H., & van Hoof, J. (1991). Differences in the structure of social behaviour of autistic children and non-autistic retarded controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 995–1015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Biro, S., Koos, O., & Brockbank, M. (1999). Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of “pure reason” in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237–267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cuccaro, M. L., Cope, H., Donnelly, S., Wolpert, C., Gabriels, R., Wright, H. H., Abramson, R. K., Koch, A. A., Gilbert, J., & Pericak-Vance, M. A. (2004). The repetitive behavior phenotype in autism: Age-related changes. American Journal Of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 1, 101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, J. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 353–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasser, V., Ulbaek, I., & Premack, D. (1989). The perception of intention. Science, 243, 365–367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 479–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dittrich, W. H., & Lea, S. E. G. (1994). Visual perception of intentional motion. Perception, 23, 253–268.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 711–720.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happe, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Johansson, M., Gillberg, C., Dolan, R., Frackowiak, R., & Frith, C. (1996). ‘Theory of mind’ in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. Neuroreport: An International Journal For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 8, 197–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hashimoto, H. (1966). A phenomenal analysis of social perception. Journal of Child Development, 2, 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: The Social Attribution Task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41, 831–846.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P.C., Pickles, A., Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview–revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. Oxford: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miles, J. H., Takahashi, N., & Mudrick, J. D. (2000). Repetitive behaviors differentiate autism subgroups. American Journal of Human Genetics, 67, 588.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. G., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, P. W. (1997). Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non-autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 401–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949–971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. (1989). Mullen scales of early learning. Cranston, RI.: T.O.T.A.L. Child, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Neal, R. A. (2001). Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a transactional social-orienting model of autism. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation: Autism (pp. 139–168). San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989). The theoretical implications of joint-attention deficits in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 173–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 27, 657–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D. (1990). The infant’s theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition, 36, 1–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ricks, D. M., & Wing, L. (1975). Language, communication, and the use of symbols in normal and autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 5, 191–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rime, B. (1985). The perception of interpersonal emotions originated by patterns of movement. Motivation and Emotion, 9, 241–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P., Morgan, R., & Carpenter, M. (1997). Young infants’ sensitivity to movement information specifying social causality. Cognitive Development, 12, 441–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1988). Neuropsychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 201–221.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M. D., & Rogers, S. J. (2003). The cognitive underpinnings of pretend play in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(3), 289–302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M. D., Young, G. S., Hepburn, S., & Rogers, S. (in press). A longitudinal study of pretend play in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

  • Scambler, D. J., Rogers, S. J., Rutherford, M. D., & Wehner, E. A. (in press). Emotional responsivity in children with autism and other developmental delays. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

  • Scholl, B. J., & Tremoulet, P. (2000). Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 299–309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tremoulet, P., & Feldman, J. (2000). Perception of animacy from the motion of a single object. Perception, 29, 943–951.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play and language comprehension in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 20, 318–337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wheaton, K. J., Pipingas, A., Silberstein, R. B., & Puce, A. (2001). Human neural responses elicited to observing the actions of others. Visual Neuroscience, 18, 401–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to M. D. Rutherford.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rutherford, M.D., Pennington, B.F. & Rogers, S.J. The Perception of Animacy in Young Children with Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 36, 983–992 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0136-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0136-8

Keywords

Navigation