Introduction

Prenatal exposure to tobacco is associated with systematic growth restriction of the offspring, including low birth weight and a smaller head circumference [1]. Epidemiologic studies, including results from our own cohort, suggest that these growth-restricted offspring go on to experience cognitive and behavioral problems in early childhood [2,3,4,5]. These findings are supported by animal models, which show that prenatal exposure to nicotine is associated with hyperactivity, decreased attention, and mild learning deficits in the offspring [6,7,8].

The neurocognitive burden of prenatal exposure to tobacco is concerning, as many tobacco exposures during pregnancy are involuntary. Estimates from 2014 suggest that nearly one in four non-smoking adults of childbearing age experience secondhand exposure to tobacco [9]. Thus, there is a need to identify individual-level, nutritional factors that may mitigate the adverse programming caused by prenatal exposures to tobacco. Identification of such mitigating factors may increase our understanding of potential interventions to minimize tobacco-induced risks to the offspring. Furthermore, identifying potential effect modifiers may support the hypothesized biological mechanisms, which may include structural changes to the developing fetal brain [10] or DNA methylation changes [11].

One such factor is food folate or folic acid [12]. On its own, higher plasma folate during pregnancy is associated with improved attention [13], whereas lower plasma folate is associated with hyperactivity [14] and emotional problems [15]. Folate plays an important role in the synthesis of methionine (a key methyl donor for DNA methylation) [16, 17] and may counteract DNA methylation changes induced by prenatal exposure to tobacco [18].

Similarly, the positive impacts of breastfeeding on offspring neurodevelopment are numerous [19]. Breast milk is rich in nutrients that may confer positive benefits to fetal brain development, such as increased myelination, brain volume, and cortical thickness.[20] Indeed, a longer duration of exclusive breastfeeding has been independently associated with improved cognitive development in early childhood [21]. Yet, these positive attributes may be offset by lactational exposure to tobacco, particularly if the mother is a smoker [22]. Thus, it is unclear whether breastfeeding would mitigate or augment the cognitive and behavioral risks associated with prenatal exposure to tobacco.

We utilized data from Healthy Start, a robust cohort of mother-infant pairs living in Colorado followed from the early prenatal period through early childhood, to explore whether there is an interactive effect between prenatal exposure to tobacco and early-life nutrition on offspring cognitive and behavioral health. We hypothesized that higher folate intake during pregnancy and a longer duration of breastfeeding would protect against the adverse effects of prenatal exposure to tobacco on offspring cognitive and behavioral health.

Methods

Healthy Start is a prospective cohort of 1410 ethnically diverse pregnant people who delivered at University of Colorado Hospital. Study participants were pregnant people (≥ 16 years) who were patients at obstetrics clinics at the University of Colorado Hospital (< 24 weeks gestation). Exclusion criteria for study participation included: multiple gestation pregnancies; previous stillbirth or preterm birth at < 25 weeks gestation; preexisting diabetes; asthma; cancer; or a previous psychiatric illness. Participants were invited to participate in two in-person research visits during pregnancy (median: 17 and 27 weeks gestation), one soon after delivery, and one at age 5 years (range: 4–7 years). For our analyses, mother-child pairs were included if the child was born > 37 weeks gestation and > 2500 g at birth.

Written informed consent was obtained from the mother or legal guardian of the child prior to each research visit. Protocols for enrollment and biospecimen collection were approved by the Colorado Multiple Institutional Review Board (#09-0563). The protocol for the current analysis was approved by the University of Texas Health Science Center Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects (HSC-SPH-20-0080).

Prenatal Exposure to Tobacco

Prenatal exposure to tobacco was assessed via urinary cotinine (major metabolite of nicotine) at ~ 27 weeks gestation. Cotinine was previously analyzed in stored urine samples via solid phase competitive ELISA with a sensitivity of 1 ng/mL (Calbiotech Cotinine ELISA CO096D, Calbiotech, El Cajon, California). Consistent with a previous Healthy Start analysis [5], cotinine concentrations were dichotomized as no exposure (< 0.05 ng/mL; the limit of detection [LOD]) and prenatal exposure to exposure (> LOD).

Childhood Cognition

The National Institutes of Health (NIH) Toolbox Cognition Battery is a series of measures used to assess executive function across the lifespan (3–85 years) [23]. A series of tests were administered to children via computer tablets and supervised at each in-person visit by professional research assistants. Measures with the most relevance to our study population included the Flanker test (for assessing inhibitory control), the Dimensional Change Card Sort test (DCCS, for assessing cognitive flexibility), and the Picture Vocabulary Test (for assessing receptive language). Raw scores from these assessments were based on both accuracy and response time (for the Flanker and DCCS) or accuracy (for the picture vocabulary test). Fully corrected T-scores accounted for age, sex, race/ethnicity and maternal education and were standardized to have a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 for all tests [24]. Higher scores reflect better cognitive performance.

Childhood Behavior

The preschool version of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (24) was captured at the 5-year visit among Healthy Start participants. Parents (or caretakers) were instructed to rate their child’s behaviors and social competencies with responses recorded on a Likert Scale as: 0 = Not True; 1 = Somewhat or Sometimes True; 2 = Very True or Often True. The CBCL scoring system groups behaviors that commonly occurring together as follows: (1) ‘broad band’ composite scales, including externalizing problems (summing scores over rule-breaking and aggressive behaviors), internalizing problems (summing scores for anxious/depressed; withdrawn/depressed; somatic complaints), and a total problems category; (2) empirically-based syndrome scales; and (3) diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) oriented scales. Fully corrected T-scores accounted for age and sex and were standardized to have a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 for all tests, with higher scores reflecting more deviant behavior. T-scores ≥ 64 on the composite scales and T-scores ≥ 70 on the syndrome/DSM scales are considered to be in the clinical range for referral to mental health evaluations, with borderline evaluations ranging from 60 to 63 and 65–69 on the composite and syndrome/DSM scales respectively) [25, 26].

Maternal Folate Intake During Pregnancy

Maternal diet was assessed via the Automated Self-Administered 24-Hour Dietary Recall web-based tool during the pregnancy period. Monthly calls were made throughout the pregnancy (range of 2–8 calls). Individual nutrients were determined using the Nutrition Data System for Research (NDSR) software, at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill’s Nutrition and Obesity Research Center. Natural folate and synthetic folic acid from enriched foods were used to determine dietary folate equivalents (DFEs), which account for differences in the absorption of naturally occurring food folate and the more bioavailable synthetic folic acid. Folic acid from dietary supplements was measured by querying brand, type, and dose of supplements used during pregnancy, as previously described [27]. Total maternal folate intake (µg/day) was determined by combining DFE intake (µg/day) and folic acid from supplements (µg/day) in a single folate variable, representing folate from all sources (foods, enriched foods, and supplements). The suggested folate requirement for pregnant people, from food and supplements, is ~ 520 µg/day [28]. Less than 5% of our study population fell below this point—which precluded interaction analyses using this clinical cut-point. Thus, for purposes of this study, maternal intake of folate was assessed using a 25th percentile cut-point (1074 µg/day), a cut-point that has been shown to mitigate the risk of tobacco-induced adverse birth outcomes in previous Healthy Start analysis [29].

Duration of Exclusive Breastfeeding

At the 5 months in-person visit, study participants were asked whether they were: (i) currently feeding their infant any breast milk; (ii) had ever fed their infant formula; or (iii) were currently feeding their infant formula. For this analysis, the duration of exclusive breastfeeding variable was dichotomized as exclusively breastfed from birth to age 5 months (if they answered ‘yes’ to the first question and ‘no’ to the remaining questions) and not exclusively breastfed (if they indicated mixed or formula feeding).

Covariates

Gestational age was determined based on a mother’s reported date of birth and estimated date of delivery. At enrollment, mothers reported their highest level of education, household income, race, ethnicity, and pre-pregnancy height and weight. At the 5-months visit, mothers were asked to report the number of adults in the household (including themselves) who were regular smokers. Responses to this question ranged from 0 to 6. We dichotomized these data into no household smokers versus any household smokers. Cotinine was measured in childhood urine samples collected at the 5-years follow up visit. Childhood cotinine was dichotomized as no exposure (cotinine < LOD) and any exposure (cotinine ≥ LOD).

Statistical Analysis

Multivariable linear regression models examined the main effects of maternal folate intake (</≥ 1074 µg/day [the 25th percentile]), breastfeeding duration (</≥5months), and prenatal exposure to tobacco (< LOD, ≥ LOD) on cognitive and behavioral outcomes. Interaction by early-life nutrition was assessed by including a product term between prenatal exposure to tobacco and the dichotomized folate or breastfeeding variables in separate regression models. Confounders were selected based on directed acyclic graphs (DAGS) and previous literature findings. All of our models adjusted for the following covariates: maternal age (years), maternal education (< 12 years; high school degree; any college), maternal race and ethnicity (Hispanic; non-Hispanic Black; non-Hispanic White; all other races and ethnicities combined), postnatal exposure to tobacco (cotinine < LOD, cotinine ≥ LOD), and infant sex. We present adjusted beta coefficients and corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Statistical significance was set at an alpha level of 0.05 for the main effects models and 0.10 for the interaction models. All statistical analyses were performed using SAS© OnDemand for Academics.

Results

Within the entire Healthy Start cohort (n = 1410), the majority of mothers were non-Hispanic white, had household incomes > $70,000, and some college education. These characteristics were similar within the analytic sample for the behavioral outcomes (n = 366). The analytic sample for the cognitive outcomes (n = 189) included mothers who were slightly older, had slightly lower mean daily folate levels, and reported a longer duration of exclusive breastfeeding (results not presented).

Compared to mothers with cotinine levels indicating active smoking or secondhand exposure to tobacco, mothers with no exposure to tobacco were older, had higher household incomes and higher levels of education, consumed more folate during pregnancy, birthed larger babies, breastfed their offspring for a longer duration, and were more likely to be Hispanic or non-Hispanic white (Table 1).

Table 1 Characteristics of mother-child pairs according to urinary cotinine in pregnancy by neuro-behavioral sub-samples, Healthy Start (2010–2014)

Our main effects models revealed that prenatal exposure to tobacco was moderately associated with decreased inhibitory control (beta coefficient: − 3.2; 95% CI: − 6.8, 0.3; p = 0.07), lower maternal folate intakes were associated with higher anxious/depressed T-scores (beta coefficient: 1.1; 95% CI: 0.1, 2.1; p = 0.04), and a shorter duration of breastfeeding (< 5 months) was associated with lower receptive language scores (beta coefficient: − 4.3; 95% CI: − 8.5, 0.0; p = 0.05) (Table 2).

Table 2 Adjusted beta coefficients for maternal prenatal folate intake, breastfeeding duration, and prenatal cotinine categories and selected neuro-cognitive and behavioral outcomes, Healthy Start (2010–2014)

The interaction results indicate that the duration of breastfeeding modified the association between prenatal exposure to tobacco with sleep problems (p for interaction = 0.01), depressive problems (p for interaction = 0.02), and total problems (p for interaction = 0.09) (Table 3) To

Table 3 Adjusted beta coefficients for maternal cotinine categories and fully adjusted T-Scores for selected cognitive and neuro-cognitive and behavioral outcomes by prenatal daily folate intake and breastfeeding duration, Healthy Start (2010–2014)

bacco-exposed offspring had more sleep problems (beta: 3.8; 95% CI: 0.5, 7.1; interaction p-value = 0.02), depressive problems (beta: 4.6; 95% CI: 1.0, 8.2; interaction p-value = 0.01), and total problems (beta: 5.8; 95% CI: − 0.7, 12.4; interaction p-value = 0.09) if they were exclusively breastfed for at least 5 months, but not for shorter durations. We found no evidence of an interaction between prenatal exposure to tobacco and folate with any of the other behavioral or cognitive outcomes.

Discussion

In this exploratory analysis, we found some indications that a shorter duration of breastfeeding, lower maternal folate intakes, and prenatal exposure to tobacco were associated with adverse cognitive and behavioral traits in early childhood. Our interaction results further revealed that the combination of prenatal exposure to tobacco and a longer duration of breastfeeding was associated with more behavioral problems in early childhood, which may be due to lactational exposure to tobacco byproduct. This key finding supports the need for smoking cessation efforts beginning in pregnancy and throughout postpartum period (when mothers are encouraged to breastfeed) to minimize neurobehavioral risks to the offspring. These preliminary findings should be followed up in larger prospective cohorts.

A shorter duration of breastfeeding was associated with decreased receptive language, which mirrors findings by Oddy and colleagues [30]. Yet, we found no other evidence that exclusive breastfeeding influenced any of the other cognitive or behavioral outcomes. This may be somewhat expected, given that a recent meta-analysis of 80 epidemiologic studies described how many of the positive effects of breastfeeding on childhood neurodevelopment disappeared after controlling for confounders, such as maternal cognitive and socioeconomic status [31].

The American Academy of Pediatrics’ recommends that infants be exclusively breastfed for at least six months, regardless of tobacco use or exposure [32]. Consistent with this recommendation, we hypothesized that a longer duration of breastfeeding would mitigate the effects of prenatal exposure to tobacco on adverse cognitive and behavioral outcomes, despite potential lactational exposure to tobacco byproducts. Yet, we found the opposite: a longer period of breastfeeding combined with prenatal exposure to tobacco was associated with more sleep problems, depressive problems, and total problems in early childhood. How these exposures, which occur in the fetal and early postnatal periods, influence sleep at age 5 years is not clear. Offspring with prenatal exposure to tobacco may also experience postnatal exposure to tobacco, particularly if the mother is a smoker. Greater doses of nicotine delivered to infants via breastmilk appeared to disrupt infant sleep cycles [33], which may be a result of reduced breast milk supply [34], changes in breast milk composition [35], or the stimulating effects of nicotine [36]. Sleep problems in early infancy may persist for many years [37]. These sleep disruptions may ultimately contribute to behavior problems [38] and depressive symptoms [39] in childhood, which further supports our finding that prenatal exposure to tobacco and a longer duration of breastfeeding was associated with more problem behaviors overall.

A majority (95%) of the pregnant people in our study had folate levels adequate for minimizing neural tube defect risk (> 520 µg/day). Even so, higher folate intakes during pregnancy were associated with increased anxious/depressed symptoms in the 5-year-old offspring. However, contrary to other observational studies [13, 14] and a recent clinical trial [40], folate intake did not appear to influence child cognition or other behavioral outcomes. It is important to note that we excluded offspring born prior to 37 weeks, as even moderate preterm birth (32–37 weeks) increases the risk for developmental delays [41]. Only one other study excluded these high-risk offspring and reported no association between folate and infant cognitive development [42]. Furthermore, our null findings could be partially attributed to folate intake being determined via self-report of diet and supplements (whereas previously published studies have used biomarkers to determine folate status [13, 14, 40]), as well as our small sample (n = 189), which may have limited our ability to detect subtle changes in child cognition and behavior.

Furthermore, we found no evidence of an interaction between folate intake and tobacco on childhood cognition or behavior, which may point to distinct and unrelated biological mechanisms. For instance, folate may impact childhood cognition and behavior via homocysteine pathways [43, 44], whereas prenatal exposure to tobacco may contribute to adverse cognitive and behavioral traits in children via overstimulation of fetal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors [45] or tobacco-induced fetal hypoxia [46], resulting in long-lasting detriments to brain morphology [10]. Whether epigenetics play a role warrants further investigation [11].

Our study may be limited by the neurobehavioral measures used to assess childhood cognition and behavior. Although the CBCL is being increasingly used in clinical settings, it may fail to identify certain mental disorders [47] or capture ‘episodicity’ (i.e. the occurrence of sporadic or irregular events) [48], which are common among children with certain behavioral problems [49]. Similarly, the validity of the NIH toolbox among younger children (ages 3–6 years) has been difficult to assess due to the absence of a gold standard for targeted constructs for these ages.

An important limitation of our approach is inability to determine lactational exposures to tobacco in breast milk. Because Healthy Start did not collect breast milk samples, we lack data on chemical or nutritional composition of breast milk. Furthermore, mothers were not explicitly asked about their tobacco use or exposure while breastfeeding. Similarly, we lack data on diet and supplements during the periconceptional period (three months prior to conception through the second month of pregnancy). This represents a critical developmental window in which folate may have a profound effect on child neurodevelopment [50] and substantially lower autism risk [51, 52]. Thus, folate intake during this period may have the greatest benefit for reducing the cognitive and behavioral burden of tobacco-exposed. Future prospective studies are needed to explore this important area of research.

Small numbers in analytic samples—particularly in the examinations of low folate and tobacco-exposed groups—may have hindered our ability to detect meaningful associations or interactions in some of our outcome groups. Even so, we cannot rule out the potential for chance findings given that we performed 54 separate main effects analyses and 38 separate interaction analyses. Correction for multiple testing (e.g. Bonferroni set to p < 0.003 = 0.10 / 38 for the interaction analyses) would impose a severe penalty on the results, many of which would drop out of statistical significance.

One of the primary strengths of this study included the use of cotinine to objectively measure tobacco exposure both during the prenatal and postnatal periods. Second, the use of the ASA24 online platform to capture total daily folate intake (which was utilized at multiple time points throughout pregnancy) reduced the potential for reporting errors and recall bias in our examinations. Lastly, following mother-child pairs from the prenatal period through the first few years of life (for the participating children) is a unique strength of Healthy Start—providing insight into little-studied associations between the early fetal period and later neurobehavioral outcomes with important preventative care implications.

Summary

Prenatal exposure to tobacco is associated with adverse cognitive and behavioral traits in childhood [2,3,4,5]. Certain nutritional factors, such as folate intake during pregnancy or breastfeeding, may modify the adverse effects of prenatal exposure to tobacco. However, since breast milk may contain tobacco byproducts [22], it is unclear whether breastfeeding would offset or augment the cognitive and behavioral risks induced by prenatal exposure to tobacco. In this exploratory analysis, we examined whether higher intakes of folate during pregnancy or a longer duration of exclusive breastfeeding modified the associations between prenatal exposure to tobacco and childhood cognition/behavior. The combination of prenatal exposure to tobacco and a longer duration of exclusive breastfeeding was associated with more sleep problems, depressive problems, and total problems in early childhood. While there are many public health benefits to longer durations of breastfeeding (e.g. reduced risk for obesity) [53], it remains important to encourage pregnant people to quit smoking and make efforts to avoid secondhand exposure to tobacco to limit adverse cognitive and behavioral outcomes in the offspring.