Skip to main content
Log in

Emotion, rationality, and social identity: a theoretical–methodological proposal for a cognitive approach

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Humberto Maturana's biology of cognition and the studies of neuroscientist Antonio Damasio have proposed theories that indicate the role of emotion in cognitive processes. Emotion lies at the basis of any behavior. An emotional transition defines a transition from one domain of actions to the other, while emotions shared in human interaction define collective domains of action and thus also social identity. In this case, they may give rise to operational coherences within the same domain of action which is recognized as “rationality”. The transition from one operational coherence to another within the same rationality does not therefore entail an emotional transition. This article uses this theoretical framework to propose an interpretation of the phenomenon of social identity and distinguishes social identities based only on emotions from those related to operational coherence. We call the former type of social identity “ontological”, while the latter is referred to as “formal”. To empirically prove the theoretical proposal of the article, the concept of metacontrast found in social self-categorization theory and its quantification elaborated by Alberto Voci are used. According to the social self-categorization theory, the greater the distance between two groups’ visions of the world, the greater the metacontrast between them. Using our model, metacontrast must thus be greater for ontological social identity and less for formal social identity. Our hypothesis was confirmed by the data. This outcome allows huge applications of the methodology proposed, as exemplified in the final considerations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Maturana alternatively uses the words “behavior” and “action” with a seemingly similar meaning within his theory. In this article, we will only use the word “action” to improve the internal coherence of the text.

  2. Translated by the author from the original in Spanish.

References

  • Baudrillard J (1998) The consumer society: myths and structures. Sage, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger J, Heath C (2007) Where consumers diverge from others: Identity signaling and product domains. J Consum Res 34:121–134

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blau PM (1956) Bureaucracy in modern society. Random House, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Blau PM, Scott RW (1962) Formal organization. Chandler Publishing, San Francisco

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer MB (1991) The social self: on being the same and different at the same time. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 17(5):475–482

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer MB, Pierce KP (2005) Social identity complexity and outgroup tolerance. Person Soc Psychol Bull 31(3):428–436

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer MB, Manzi JM, Shaw JS (1993) In-group identification as a function of depersonalization, distinctiveness and status. Psychol Sci 4(2):88–92

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan K, Prendergast G (2007) Materialism and social comparison among adolescents. Soc Behav Person Int J 35(2):213–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney-Lippold J (2011) A new algorithmic identity: soft biopolitics and the modulation of control. Theo Cult Soc 28(6):164–181. https://doi.org/10.1177/0263276411424420

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Childers TL, Rao AR (1992) The influence of familial and peer-based reference groups on consumer decision. J Cons Res 19:198–211

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J (1970) Approximate power and sample size determination for common one-sample and two-sample hypothesis tests. Edu Psychol Measur 30(4):811–831

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New Jersey

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooley CH (1962) Social organization: a study of the larger mind. Schocken Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio AR (1994) Descartes’ error: emotion, reason and the human brain. Avan Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio AR (1999) The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Harcourt Brace, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Durkheim E (1933) The division of labor in society. The Free Press, Glencoe, Illinois

    Google Scholar 

  • Escalas JE, Bettman JR (2005) Self-construal, reference groups, and brand meaning. J Consum Res 32(3):378–389

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giddens A (1990) The consequences of modernity. Polity Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Haslam SA, Turner JC, Oakes PJ, Reynolds KJ, Doosje B (2004) From personal pictures in the head to collective tools in the world: how shared stereotypes allow groups to represent and change social reality. In: McGarty C, Yzerbyt VY, Spears R (eds) Stereotypes as explanations the formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayles NK (2014) Cognition everywhere: the rise of the cognitive nonconscious and the costs of consciousness. New Lit Hist 45(2):199–220. https://doi.org/10.1353/nlh.2014.0011

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lachance MJ, Beaudoin P, Robitaille J (2003) Adolescents’ brand sensitivity in apparel: influence of three socialization agents. Int J Consum Stud 27(1):47–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lash S (2007) Power after hegemony: cultural studies in mutation? Theo Cult Soc 24(3):55–78. https://doi.org/10.1177/0263276407075956

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maturana HR (1978) Biology of language: the epistemology of reality. In: Miller GA, Elizabeth L (eds) Psychology and biology of language and thought: essays in Honor of Eric Lenneberg. Academic Press, New York, pp 27–63

    Google Scholar 

  • Maturana HR (1988a) Reality: the search for objectivity or the quest for a compelling argument. Irish J Psychol 9(1):25–82. https://doi.org/10.1080/03033910.1988.10557705

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, HR (1988b) Ontology for observing: the biological foundations of self consciousness and the physical domain of existence. In: Proceedings of the Conference on Texts in Cybernetic Theory, American Society for Cybernetics, Felton, CA, pp 18–23. https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/b197/549e065158d8102ca973ae705ad8f0082e14.pdf

  • Maturana HR (1990a) Science and daily life: the ontology of scientific explanations. In: Krohn W, Koppers G, Nowotny H (eds) Selforganization: portrait of a scientific revolution. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, pp 12–35

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Maturana HR (1990b) Biologia de la Cognicion y Epistemologia. Universidad de la Frontera, Temuco, Chile. https://es.slideshare.net/Longsthride/biologia-de-la-cognicion-y-epistemologia-humberto-maturana

  • Maturana HR (1995) Biology of self-consciousness. In: Trautteur G (ed) Consciousness: distinction and reflection. Bibliopolis, Naples

    Google Scholar 

  • Maturana HR (2001) Emociones y Lenguaje en Educación y Política. Santiago de Chile: Ediciones Dolmen. https://des-juj.infd.edu.ar/sitio/upload/Maturana_Romesin_H_-_Emociones_Y_Lenguaje_En_Educacion_Y_Politica.pdf

  • Maturana HR, Varela FJ (1980) Autopoiesis and cognition. Reidel Publishing Company, London

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Maturana HR, Varela FJ (1987) The tree of knowledge: The biological roots of human understanding. Shambhala, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead GH (1934) Mind, self, and society. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Merton RK (1968) Social theory and social structure. Free Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Rindfleisch A, Burroughs JE, Denton F (1997) Family structure, materialism, and compulsive consumption. J Consum Res 23:312–325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts JA, Manolis C, Tanner JF Jr (2008) Interpersonal influence and adolescent materialism and compulsive buying. Soc Influen 3(2):114–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nist Sematech (2013). Sample size required. In: Engineering statistics handbook. https://www.itl.nist.gov/div898/handbook/prc/section2/prc222.htm

  • Shim S, Koh A (1997) Profiling adolescent consumer decision-making styles: effects of socialization agents and social-structural variables. Cloth Textil Res J 15(1):50–59

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmel G (1957) Fashion. Am J Sociol 62(6):541–558

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Timmor Y, Katz-Navon T (2008) Being the same and different: a model explaining new product adoption. J Consum Behav 7(3):249–262

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tönnies F (1957) Community and society. Harper and Row, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Totaro P, Marinho TA (2019) The duality of social self-categorization in consumption. J Consum Cult 19(2):189–212. https://doi.org/10.1177/1469540517717774

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Totaro P, Ninno D (2020) Biological recursion and digital systems: conceptual tools for analysing man-machine interaction. Theo Cult Soc 37(5):27–49. https://doi.org/10.1177/0263276420915264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner JC, Hogg MA, Oakes PJ, Reicher SD, Wetherell MS (1987) Rediscovering the social group: a self-categorization theory. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner JC, Oakes PJ, Haslam SA, McGarty C (1994) Self and collective: cognition and social context. Person Soc Psychol Bull 20(5):454–463

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voci A (2006) Relevance of social categories, depersonalization and group processes: two field tests of self-categorization theory. Eur J Soc Psychol 36(1):73–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber M (1949) The methodology of Social Science. The Free Press, Glencoe, Illinois

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber M (1978) Economy and society. University of California Press, Berkeley

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank “Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior” for its financial support; the “Centro di Studi sul Rischio” (founded by Niklas Luhmann and Raffaele De Giorgi, also its current director) for its logistical and scientific support; the coordinators and the teaching staff of the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences bachelor courses, University of Salento (Lecce, Italy), Cosimo Caputo, Professor of General Semiotics, Department of Human Sciences, University of Salento (Lecce, Italy) and Prof. Antonio Errico, Principal of the Liceo Quinto Ennio of Gallipoli (Lecce, Italy) for having made it possible to conduct empirical investigations by administering questionnaires to students. Finally, I am indebted to the referees of Cognitive Processing for their helpful suggestions that have contributed to improving the quality of this article.

Funding

This study was funded by “Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior” – Brazil—(grant number: 88881.120206/2016–01).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paolo Totaro.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The author Paolo Totaro declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

This appendix is identical to appendix A presented by Voci (2006: 89–90), except for the context to which the technique is applied, and is included here for the convenience of the reader.

As explained in the Method section, each interviewee was asked to imagine 100 consumers of a brand he or she liked (in-group) and 100 consumers of a brand he or she did not like (out-group). The interviewee was then asked to distribute both groups along a metric 5-point scale based on the degree to which these imaginary consumers belong to consensual domains of actions. For the first study, the domains were Differentiation, Family, and Friendship. For the second study, the domains of Differentiation, Assimilation, Friendship, Family, and Social Network were used.

One participant generated the following distribution for Friendship: 0–0-10–90-0 for the in-group and 0–40-60–0-0 for the out-group. Thus, 10 in-group members are situated in the third cell of the distribution and 90 in the fourth. Concerning the out-group, 40 members are in the second cell and 60 in the third. If we consider the first 10 in-group members, they are different by 1 position from the 40 out-group members and by 0 position from the 60 out-group members. Thus, we have 10 times 40 with a difference of 1 and 10 times 60 with a difference of 0. The other 90 in-group members are 2 positions distant from the 40 out-group members and 1 position from the 30 out-group members. Thus, we have 90 times 40 with a difference of 2, and 90 times 60 with a difference of 1. Inter-group differences are thus \(10 \times 40 + 10 \times 60 \times 0 + 90 \times 40 \times 2 + 90 \times 60 = 13000.\) The total number of comparisons made is: \(10 \times 40 + 10 \times 60 + 90 \times 40 + 90 \times 60 = 10000.\) Thus, average inter-group differences are \(\frac{13000}{{10000}} = 1.30\). In the computation of intra-group differences, only the distribution concerning the in-group is involved. In this case, we have 10 members who are different by 1 position from 90 members. Thus, the differences within the in-group are: \(10 \times 90 = 900\). For the intra-group comparison we have: \(\frac{{\left[ {100 \times \left( {100 - 1} \right)} \right]}}{2} = 4950\); average intra-group differences are \(\frac{900}{{4950}} = 0.18.\) After the constant 1 is added to both terms, the meta-contrast ratio corresponds to: \(\frac{2.30}{{1.18}} = 1.94\).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Totaro, P. Emotion, rationality, and social identity: a theoretical–methodological proposal for a cognitive approach. Cogn Process 22, 579–592 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-021-01030-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-021-01030-9

Keywords

Navigation