Skip to main content
Log in

Preschool children’s conversational skills for explaining game rules: communicative guidance strategies as a function of type of relationship and gender

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Ten trios of children from 4 to 6 years old were observed in a situation where one child (the expert) who had learned the rules of a game explained these rules to two other children at the same time (the novices): one with whom s/he had a positive relationship and the other with whom her/his relationship was negative. Within this asymmetrical situation created artificially, the children functioned on the basis of a complex tutorial contract. The results indicated that, at these young ages, children are capable to strongly manage three dimensions of the explanatory goal: interactional, ideational (management of the object), and linguistic. However, the errors made by the novices were regulated differently, depending on the type of relationship and gender: the experts in boy trios intervened less frequently when errors were made by the novice with whom the relationship was negative (i.e., the not-friend novice) than with the other novice; conversely, the experts in girl trios intervened less frequently when errors were produced by the novice with whom the relationship was positive (i.e., the friend novice) than with the other novice. An analysis of the communicative strategies observed here highlights early sophisticated pragmatic skills in this interactive assigned design.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. “Recognition in the term ‘explicit recognition’ refers to recognition of the other’s representation of the task and not the social aspects of recognition that we also intend to explore in the present paper, although, of course, the two types of recognition are inextricably intertwined” (Psaltis and Duveen 2007, p. 82).

  2. During the game, girls answered 13 out of 19 questions (68.42 %) from friend novices and 14 out of 16 questions (87.50 %) from not-friend novices; boys answered 27 out of 29 questions (93.10 %) from friend novices and 13 out of 14 (92.85 %) questions from not-friend novices.

  3. Original language: 5 SAR: tu commences [^ SAR gives the die to the not-friend girl novice]; 12 CLEM: c’est toi qui commences Estelle [= friend girl novice]; 3 FLO: alors Charles tu lances le dé et si tu es bleu tu vas là et si tu as vert tu peux commencer [= friend boy novice]; 5 MAR (girl expert); and 10 GUI (boy expert): alors qui c’est qui commence?

  4. In Clementine A's trio (4; 9 years), the expert often said that the novices did not understand anything. Criticism was explicitly addressed once to the not-friend novice: ‘you didn't understand anything.” In Melanie's trio, a criticism is brought to the not-friend novice's attention too: “you are doing just any old thing.”

  5. Among 12 interventions of the rule recall type, 9 used a deontic term, and 3 used a verb in the present tense.

  6. We thank the reviewer for this suggestion.

References

  • Baker, M., Joab, M., Safar, B., & Schlienger, D. (2000). Analyses et modélisations d’explications dans un corpus de dialogues finalisés. Psychologie de l’Interaction, 9(10), 7–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banks-Leite, L. (1999). Langue et argumentation: considérations sur les échanges linguistiques à l’école maternelle. Psychologie de l’Interaction, 7(8), 81–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barbieri, M. S., Colavita, F., & Scheuer, N. (1989). Explanations: a pragmatic basis for early child competence. Papers in Pragmatics, 3(1), 129–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaudichon, J. (1982). La communication sociale chez l’enfant. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaudichon, J. (1999). La communication: processus, formes et applications. Paris: A. Colin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berndt, T. J. (1987). The distinctive features of conversations between friends: theories, research and implications for socio-moral development. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Moral development through social interaction (pp. 281–300). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blum-Kulka, S., Hamo, M., & Habib, T. (2010). Explanations in naturally occurring peer talk: conversational emergence and function, thematic scope, and contribution to the development of discursive skills. First Language, 30(3–4), 440–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brixhe, D. (1990). Les interactions entre élèves dans la construction des discours explicatifs. Psychologie Française, 71, 7–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brixhe, D., Marro Clément, P., & Picard, A.-F. (1996). Gestion interactive de séquences explicatives. Interaction & Cognitions, 1(2–3), 425–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronson, W. C. (1975). Behavior with age mates during the second year of life. In M. Lewis, G. Young, J. Brooks, & L. Michalson (Eds.), Friendship and peer relations (pp. 131–152). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn, J., & Lewis, M. (1978). Early social knowledge: the development of knowledge about others. In McGurk (Ed.), Issues in childhood social development (pp. 79–106). London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: some universals in language usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Bruxelles, S., & De Gaulmyn, M. M. (2000). Explication et interaction: facteurs déterminants et degré d’efficacité. Psychologie de l’Interaction, 9(10), 47–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carli, L. L., & Bukatko, D. (2000). Gender, communication and social influence: a developmental perspective. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 295–331). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, L. (1983). Dialogue games. Amsterdam: De Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J. (1996). Arguing with siblings, friends, and mothers: developments in relationships and understanding. In D. I. Slobin, J. Gerhardt, A. Kyratzis, & J. Guo (Eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language. Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 191–204). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ervin-Tripp, S.M. (1987). Speech acts and syntactic development: linked or independent? Keynote address to the Boston Child Language Conference (October).

  • Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (2001). The place of gender in developmental pragmatics: cultural factors. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 34(1), 131–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ghiglione, R., & Trognon, A. (1993). Où va la pragmatique? De la pragmatique à la psychologie sociale. Grenoble: Presses Universitaires de Grenoble.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilly, M., & Deblieux, M. (1999). Analyse des médiations langagières en situation dyadique de résumé de récit. In M. Gilly, J. P. Roux, & A. Trognon (Eds.), Apprendre dans l’interaction: analyse des médiations sémiotiques (pp. 95–120). Aix-en-Provence & Nancy: Presses de l’Université de Provence & Presses Universitaires de Nancy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J.M. (1983). How children become friends? Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 48 (3), no 201.

  • Grice, P. H. (1979). Logique et conversation. Communications, 30, 57–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grize, J. B. (1996). Logique naturelle et communication. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L. (1996). Desires, beliefs and language. In P. Carruthers & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 200–220). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L. (1999). Acquiring the art of conversation: children’s developing conception of their conversation partner. In M. Bennet (Ed.), Developmental psychology: achievements and prospects (pp. 89–105). London: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartup, W. W. (1985). Relationships and their significance in cognitive development. In R. A. Hinde, A. N. Perret-Clermont, & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Social relationships and cognitive development (pp. 66–82). Oxford: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartup, W. W. (1993). Adolescents and their friends. In B. Laursen (Ed.), Close friendships in adolescence (pp. 3–22). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartup, W. W. (1996). Cooperation, close relationships and cognitive development. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 213–237). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hood, L., & Bloom, L. (1979). What, when and how about why: a longitudinal study of early expressions of causality. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 44 (6, Serial No. 181).

  • Jisa, H., & Mariotte, D. (1991). Coconstruction dans l’interaction enfant-enfant. First International Conference: Analysis of interactions, La Baume-lès-Aix, 12–14 September.

  • Krappmann, L. (1985). The structure of peer relationships and possible effects on school achievement. In R. A. Hinde, A. N. Perret-Clermont, & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Social relationships and cognitive development (pp. 149–166). Oxford: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutnick, P., & Kington, A. (2005). Children’s friendships and learning in school: cognitive enhancement through social interaction? British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 521–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (1996). “Separate worlds for girls and boys?” Views from U.S. and Chinese mixed-sex friendship groups. In D. I. Slobin, J. Gerhardt, A. Kyratzis, & J. Guo (Eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language. Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 555–577). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kyratzis, A., Ross, T. S., & Koymen, S. B. (2010). Validating justifications in preschool girls’ and boys’ friendship group talk: implications for linguistic and socio-cognitive development. Journal of Child Language, 37, 115–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leman, P. J., & Duveen, G. (1999). Representations of authority and children’s moral reasoning. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 557–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, J. L. (1970). Fondements de la sociométrie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France (original edition, 1954).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, J., & Aboud, F. (1985). The resolution of social conflict among friends. Child Development, 56, 1009–1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newcomb, A. F., & Bagwell, C. L. (1995). Children’s friendship relations: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2), 306–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newcomb, A. F., & Brady, J. E. (1982). Mutuality in boys’ friendship relations. Child Development, 53, 392–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newcomb, A. F., Brady, J. E., & Hartup, W. W. (1979). Friendship and incentive condition as determinants of children’s task-oriented social behaviour. Child Development, 50, 878–881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelligrini, A. D., Galda, L., Flor, D., Bartini, M., & Charak, D. (1997). Close relationships, individual differences, and early literacy learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67(3), 409–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Psaltis, C., & Duveen, G. (2006). Social relations and cognitive development: the influence of conversation type and representations of gender. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 407–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Psaltis, C., & Duveen, G. (2007). Conservation and conversation types: forms of recognition and cognitive development. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 79–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rips, L. J. (1998). Reasoning and conversation. Psychological Review, 105(3), 411–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schieffelin, B. B., Ochs, E., Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (1996). The microgenesis of competence: methodology in language socialization. In Social interaction, social context, and language. Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 251–263). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, A. (1990). Pickle fights: gendered talk on preschool disputes. Discourse Processes, 13, 5–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M., & Surian, L. (2007). Conversational understanding in young children. In E. Hoff & M. Shatz (Eds.), Handbook of language development (pp. 304–323). New York: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sorsana, C. (1997). Affinités enfantines et corésolution de la tour de Hanoi. La Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 1, 51–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sorsana, C. (1999). Stratégies sociocognitives dans la résolution de la tour de Hanoi. In M. Gilly, J. P. Roux, & A. Trognon (Eds.), Apprendre dans l'interaction. Analyse des médiations sémiotiques (pp. 143–161). Aix-en-Provence & Nancy: Publications de l’Université de Provence & Presses Universitaires de Nancy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sorsana, C. (2003). Comment l’interaction coopérative rend-elle plus « savant »? Quelques réflexions concernant les conditions nécessaires au fonctionnement dialogique du conflit sociocognitif. L’Orientation Scolaire et Professionnelle, 32(3), 437–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sorsana, C. (2011). L’activité conversationnelle est-elle une fenêtre ouverte sur la pensée de soi et d’autrui? Enfance, 63(1), 69–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sorsana, C., & Musiol, M. (2005). Power and knowledge. How can rationality emerge from children's interactions in a problem-solving situation? In E. Grillo (Ed.), Power without domination. Dialogism and the empowering property of communication (pp. 161–221). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sorsana, C., & Trognon, A. (2011). Contextual determination of human thinking: about some conceptual and methodological obstacles in psychology studies. Human Development, 54(4), 204–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stambak, M., Ballion, M., Breaute, M., & Rayna, S. (1988). Jeux de fiction et interactions sociales entre jeunes enfants. In R. A. Hinde, A. N. Perret-Clermont, & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relations interpersonnelles et développement des savoirs (pp. 195–217). Cousset: Delval.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasley, S. D. (1995). The role of talk in children’s peer collaborations. Developmental Psychology, 31(2), 207–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tous Saint Marc, C. (1981). Couple amical et socialisation chez les jeunes écoliers. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tous Saint Marc, C. (1988). Les différences entre garçons et filles: approche génétique de leurs interactions. In I. Durning & R. E. Tremblay (Eds.), Relations entre enfants. Recherches et interventions éducatives (pp. 77–91). Paris: Fleurus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trognon, A. (2002). Speech acts and the logic of mutual understanding. In D. Vanderveken & S. Kubo (Eds.), Essays in speech act theory (pp. 121–133). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trognon, A., & Batt, M. (2010). Interlocutory logic: a unified framework for studying conversational interaction. In J. Streek (Ed.), New adventures in language and interaction (pp. 9–40). Bruxelles: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trognon, A., & Batt, M. (2012). Group dynamics and learning. In N. M. Seel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of sciences of learning, part 7 (pp. 1388–1391). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trognon, A., Batt, M., Bromberg, M., Sorsana, C., & Frigout, S. (2011a). Les formes logiques de la « parole-en-interaction » (Quelques bases de la logique interlocutoire). In P. Castel, M. F. Lacassagne, & E. Sales-Wuillemin (Eds.), Psychologie sociale, communication et langage. De la conception aux applications (pp. 327–348). Bruxelles: Editions De Boeck Université.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trognon, A., Batt, M., & Marchetti, E. (2011b). Le dialogisme de la rationalité dans l’ordre de l’interaction. Bulletin de psychologie, 64(5–515), 439–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trognon, A., Batt, M., Sorsana, C., & Saint Dizier de Almeida, V. (2011c). Argumentation and dialogue. In A. Trognon, M. Batt, J. Caelen, & D. Vernant (Eds.), Logical properties of dialogue (pp. 147–186). Nancy: Presses Universitaires de Nancy.

  • Trognon, A., & Sorsana, C. (2011). Grasping both the normative facts and the social positions emerging from conversation. Human Development, Letters to the Editor, 1, 11–17. November.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trognon, A., Sorsana, C., Batt, M., & Longin, D. (2008). Peer interaction and problem solving: one example of a logical-discursive analysis of a process of joint decision making. The European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5(5), 623–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veneziano, E. (1999). L’acquisition de connaissances pragmatiques: apprendre à expliquer. Parole, 9(10), 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veneziano, E. (2001). Interactional processes in the origins of the explaining capacity. In K. Nelson, A. Aksu-Koc, & C. Johnson (Eds.), Children’s language: developing narrative and discourse competence (Vol. 10, pp. 113–141). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veneziano, E., & Sinclair, H. (1995). Functional changes in early child language: the appearance of reference to the past and of explanations. Journal of Child Language, 22, 557–581.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zadunaisky-Ehrlich, S., & Blum-Kulka, S. (2010). Peer talk as a ‘double opportunity space’: the case of argumentative discourse. Discourse & Society, 21(2), 211–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajac, R., & Hartup, W. W. (1997). Friends as coworkers: research review and classroom implications. The Elementary School Journal, 98, 3–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Vivian Waltz and Maryse Noté for comments on the English style. We also would like to thank Rui Da Silva Neves for methodological advice.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christine Sorsana.

Appendix. Transcription conventions from CHILDES database

Appendix. Transcription conventions from CHILDES database

(.) pause; a longer pause between words is represented as (..), a very long pause as (…), and the exact length in seconds.

Explanations specifying the deictic identity of objects and people are noted as [= text].

Comments are noted as [^ comments].

%act introduces action without words.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sorsana, C., Guizard, N. & Trognon, A. Preschool children’s conversational skills for explaining game rules: communicative guidance strategies as a function of type of relationship and gender. Eur J Psychol Educ 28, 1453–1475 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-013-0175-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-013-0175-4

Keywords

Navigation