Skip to main content
Log in

Surprise-enhanced and technology-mediated learning: a two-country study

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognition, Technology & Work Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We develop a theoretical model of surprise-enhanced technology-mediated learning (STL) that builds on evolutionary biological ideas. The model predicts that in technology-mediated learning tasks made up of several discrete learning modules, the presence of a simulated surprise event eliciting negative emotions immediately before a discrete learning module will lead to enhanced learning associated with the module. An experiment involving 617 participants in two countries, USA and Mexico, is used to test the STL model. The participants reviewed six Web-based learning modules about terms used in international trade transactions and then took a test on what they had learned. Data from three experimental conditions were contrasted: two treatment and one control conditions. The two treatment conditions incorporated negative surprises in the form of Web-based screens immediately before Module 4. The surprise screens showed a snake in attack position and a computer malfunction warning, and were absent in the control condition. The participants in both surprise conditions performed significantly better than those in the control condition for Module 4. The learning enhancements were virtually the same regardless of country and type of surprise stimulus (snake attack or computer malfunction screen).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bera AK, Jarque CM (1981) Efficient tests for normality, homoscedasticity and serial independence of regression residuals: Monte Carlo evidence. Econ Lett 7(4):313–318

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen D, Thomsen DK (2005) Personal memories for remote historical events: accuracy and clarity of flashbulb memories related to World War II. J Exp Psychol Gen 134(2):242–257

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowen NK, Guo S (2011) Structural equation modeling. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Brown DE (1991) Human universals. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown R, Kulik J (1977) Flashbulb memories. Cognition 5(1):73–99

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cahill L (1997) The neurobiology of emotionally influenced memory implications for understanding traumatic memory. Ann N Y Acad Sci 821(1):238–246

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Charoensukmongkol P (2014) Effects of support and job demands on social media use and work outcomes. Comput Hum Behav 36(1):340–349

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Connolly TM, Boyle EA, MacArthur E, Hainey T, Boyle JM (2012) A systematic literature review of empirical evidence on computer games and serious games. Comput Educ 59(2):661–686

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daniel JL, Chatelain-Jardon R (2013) Enhancing workplace spirituality through emotional intelligence. J Appl Manag Entrep 18(4):3–17

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Deming DJ, Goldin C, Katz LF, Yuchtman N (2015) Can online learning bend the higher education cost curve?. National Bureau of Economic Research, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • DeRosa DM, Hantula DA, Kock N, D’Arcy J (2004) Trust and leadership in virtual teamwork: a media naturalness perspective. Hum Resour Manag 43(2–3):219–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edery-Halpern G, Nachson I (2004) Distinctiveness in flashbulb memory: comparative analysis of five terrorist attacks. Memory 12(2):147–157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frintrop S, Rome E, Christensen HI (2010) Computational visual attention systems and their cognitive foundations: a survey. ACM Trans Appl Percept 7(1):1–46

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geisser S (1974) A predictive approach to the random effects model. Biometrika 61(1):101–107

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Gel YR, Gastwirth JL (2008) A robust modification of the Jarque–Bera test of normality. Econ Lett 99(1):30–32

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg D (2005) Flashbulb memories. Skeptic 11(3):74–80

    Google Scholar 

  • Gürhan-Canli Z, Maheswaran D (2000) Determinants of country-of-origin evaluations. J Consum Res 27(1):96–108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamann S (2001) Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends Cogn Sci 5(9):394–400

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede G (2001) Culture’s consequences: comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations. Sage, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarque CM, Bera AK (1980) Efficient tests for normality, homoscedasticity and serial independence of regression residuals. Econ Lett 6(3):255–259

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Kane SM (1974) Holy Ghost people: the snake-handlers of southern Appalachia. Appalach J 1(4):255–262

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline RB (2010) Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. The Guilford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kock N (2004) The psychobiological model: towards a new theory of computer-mediated communication based on Darwinian evolution. Organ Sci 15(3):327–348

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kock N (2005) Media richness or media naturalness? The evolution of our biological communication apparatus and its influence on our behavior toward e-communication tools. IEEE Trans Prof Commun 48(2):117–130

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kock N (2009) Information systems theorizing based on evolutionary psychology: an interdisciplinary review and theory integration framework. MIS Q 33:395–418

    Google Scholar 

  • Kock N (2013) Using WarpPLS in e-collaboration studies: what if I have only one group and one condition? Int J e-Collab 9(3):1–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kock N (2014a) Advanced mediating effects tests, multi-group analyses, and measurement model assessments in PLS-based SEM. Int J e-Collab 10(3):1–13

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kock N (2014b) Using data labels to discover moderating effects in PLS-based structural equation modeling. Int J e-Collab 10(4):1–14

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kock N (2015a) WarpPLS 5.0 user manual. ScriptWarp Systems, Laredo

    Google Scholar 

  • Kock N (2015b) One-tailed or two-tailed P values in PLS-SEM? Int J e-Collab 11(2):1–7

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Kock N, Chatelain-Jardón R (2011) Four guiding principles for research on evolved information processing traits and technology-mediated task performance. J Assoc Inf Syst 12(10):684–713

    Google Scholar 

  • Kock N, Davison R (2003) Can lean media support knowledge sharing? Investigating a hidden advantage of process improvement. IEEE Trans Eng Manag 50(2):151–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kock N, Gaskins L (2014) The mediating role of voice and accountability in the relationship between Internet diffusion and government corruption in Latin America and Sub-Saharan Africa. Inf Technol Dev 20(1):23–43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kock N, Lynn GS (2012) Lateral collinearity and misleading results in variance-based SEM: an illustration and recommendations. J Assoc Inf Syst 13(7):546–580

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindell M, Whitney D (2001) Accounting for common method variance in cross-sectional research designs. J Appl Psychol 86(1):114–121

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay DG, Ahmetzanov MV (2005) Emotion, memory, and attention in the taboo Stroop paradigm. Psychol Sci 16(1):25–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maheswaran D (1994) Country of origin as a stereotype: effects of consumer expertise and attribute strength on product evaluations. J Consum Res 21(2):354–365

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maruyama GM (1998) Basics of structural equation modeling. Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Miller RB, Wichern DW (1977) Intermediate business statistics: analysis of variance, regression and time series. Holt, Rihehart and Winston, New York

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Mohanty GN, Mohanty SC (2004) Living with snakes: the life style of the Snake Charmers (Sapua Kela). ADIVASI 44(1):28–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Nasoz F, Alvarez K, Lisetti CL, Finkelstein N (2004) Emotion recognition from physiological signals using wireless sensors for presence technologies. Cogn Technol Work 6(1):4–14

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien EJ, Myers JL (1985) When comprehension difficulty improves memory for text. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 11(1):12–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Otani H, Kusumi T, Kato K, Matsuda K, Kern R, Widner R Jr, Ohta N (2005) Remembering a nuclear accident in Japan: did it trigger flashbulb memories? Memory 13(1):6–20

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne J, Jackson E, Ryan L, Hoscheidt S, Jacobs J, Nadel L (2006) The impact of stress on neutral and emotional aspects of episodic memory. Memory 14(1):1–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ranganath C, Rainer G (2003) Neural mechanisms for detecting and remembering novel events. Nat Rev Neurosci 4(3):193–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rejikumar G (2013) A pre-launch exploration of customer acceptance of usage based vehicle insurance policy. IIMB Manag Rev 25(1):19–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sapolsky RM (2003) Stress and plasticity in the limbic system. Neurochem Res 28(11):1735–1742

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sasaki J, Khalil PA, Chegondi M, Raszynski A, Meyer KG, Totapally BR (2014) Coral snake bites and envenomation in children: a case series. Pediatr Emerg Care 30(4):262–265

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schutzwohl A (1998) Surprise and schema strength. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 24(5):1182–1199

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schützwohl A, Reisenzein R (2012) Facial expressions in response to a highly surprising event exceeding the field of vision: a test of Darwin’s theory of surprise. Evol Hum Behav 33(6):657–664

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwabe L, Bohringer A, Chatterjee M, Schachinger H (2008) Effects of pre-learning stress on memory for neutral, positive and negative words: different roles of cortisol and autonomic arousal. Neurobiol Learn Mem 90(1):44–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seltzer LJ, Prososki AR, Ziegler TE, Pollak SD (2012) Instant messages vs. speech: hormones and why we still need to hear each other. Evol Hum Behav 33(1):42–45

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon AF (2006) Computer-mediated communication: task performance and satisfaction. J Social Psychol 146(3):349–379

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steizel S, Rimbau-Gilabert E (2013) Upward influence tactics through technology-mediated communication tools. Comput Hum Behav 29(2):462–472

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone M (1974) Cross-validatory choice and assessment of statistical predictions. J R Stat Soc B 36(1):111–147

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson B (2004) Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis: understanding concepts and applications. American Psychological Association, Washington

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wouters P, Van Nimwegen C, Van Oostendorp H, Van Der Spek ED (2013) A meta-analysis of the cognitive and motivational effects of serious games. J Educ Psychol 105(2):249–265

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright S (1934) The method of path coefficients. Ann Math Stat 5(3):161–215

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan RA (1996) Processing narrative time shifts. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 22(5):1196–1207

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ned Kock.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Content of the learning modules

Below are the contents of each of the six Web-based learning modules employed in the experiment discussed here. These learning modules are about terms used in international trade transactions, which are collectively referred to as Incoterms.

1.1 Module 1

The global economy has given businesses broader access than ever before to markets all over the world. Goods are sold in more countries, in larger quantities, and in greater variety. But as the volume and complexity of international sales increase, so do possibilities for misunderstandings and costly disputes when sales contracts are not adequately drafted.

Incoterms, the official International Chamber of Commerce (ICC) rules for the interpretation of trade terms, facilitate the conduct of international trade. Reference to Incoterms 2000 in a sales contract defines clearly the parties’ respective obligations and reduces the risk of legal complications.

Since the creation of Incoterms by ICC in 1936, this undisputed worldwide contractual standard has been regularly updated to keep pace with the development of international trade. Incoterms 2000 take account of the recent spread of customs-free zones, the increased use of electronic communications in business transactions, and changes in transport practices. Incoterms 2000 offer a simpler and clearer presentation of the 13 definitions, all of which have been revised.

Frequently, parties to a contract are unaware of the different trading practices in their respective countries. This can give rise to misunderstandings, disputes, and litigation, with all the waste of time and money that this entails. In order to remedy these problems, the International Chamber of Commerce first published in 1936 a set of international rules for the interpretation of trade terms. These rules were known as “Incoterms 1936.” Amendments and additions were later made in 1953, 1967, 1976, 1980, 1990, and presently in 2000 in order to bring the rules in line with current international trade practices.

1.2 Module 2

The purpose of Incoterms is to provide a set of international rules for the interpretation of the most commonly used trade terms in foreign trade. Thus, the uncertainties of different interpretations of such terms in different countries can be avoided or at least reduced to a considerable degree.

It should be stressed that the scope of Incoterms is limited to matters relating to the rights and obligations of the parties to the contract of sale with respect to the delivery of goods sold (in the sense of “tangibles,” not including “intangibles” such as computer software).

It appears that two particular misconceptions about Incoterms are very common. First, Incoterms are frequently misunderstood as applying to the contract of carriage rather than to the contract of sale. Second, they are sometimes wrongly assumed to provide for all the duties, which parties may wish to include in a contract of sale.

As has always been underlined by ICC, Incoterms deal only with the relation between sellers and buyers under the contract of sale, and, moreover, only do so in some very distinct respects.

While it is essential for exporters and importers to consider the very practical relationship between the various contracts needed to perform an international sales transaction—where not only the contract of sale is required, but also contracts of carriage, insurance, and financing—Incoterms relate to only one of these contracts, namely the contract of sale. Nevertheless, the parties’ agreement to use a particular Incoterm would necessarily have implications for the other contracts.

1.3 Module 3

The Incoterms are divided in four groups based on their arrangement. Departure is the base for group E, main carriage unpaid is the base for group F, main carriage paid is the base for group C, and arrival is the base for group D.

Group E is formed by: EXW—Ex Works (… named place). The Incoterms embraced by group F are: FCA—Free Carrier (… named place), FAS—Free Alongside Ship (… named port of shipment), and FOB—Free On Board (… named port of shipment). Group C is formed by: CFR—Cost and Freight (… named port of destination), CIF—Cost, Insurance, and Freight (… named port of destination), CPT—Carriage Paid To (… named place of destination), and CIP—Carriage and Insurance Paid To (… named place of destination). Finally, group D is formed by: DAF—Delivered At Frontier (… named place), DES—Delivered Ex Ship (… named port of destination), DEQ—Delivered Ex Quay (… named port of destination), DDU—Delivered Duty Unpaid (… named place of destination), and DDP—Delivered Duty Paid (… named place of destination).

Not all the Incoterms 2000 are appropriate for all modes of transportation. They are six that must be used exclusively when the transport is maritime and/or inland waterway. These Incoterms are FAS, FOB (group F), CFR, CIF (group C), DES and DEQ (group D). On the other hand, EXW (group E), FCA (group F), CPT, CIP (group C), and DAF, DDU, and DDP (group D) are appropriate for any mode of transportation.

1.4 Module 4

The risk of loss of or damage to the goods, as well as the obligation to bear the costs relating to the goods, passes from the seller to the buyer when the seller has fulfilled his obligation to deliver the goods. Since the buyer should not be given the possibility to delay the passing of the risk and costs, all terms stipulate that the passing of risk and costs may occur even before delivery, if the buyer does not take delivery as agreed or fails to give such instructions (with respect to time for shipment and/or place for delivery) as the seller may require in order to fulfill his obligation to deliver the goods. It is a requirement for such premature passing of risk and costs that the goods have been identified as intended for the buyer or, as is stipulated in the terms, set aside for him (appropriation).

The “E”-term requires the seller to do no more than place the goods at the disposal of the buyer at the agreed place—usually at the seller’s own premises.

The “F”-terms require the seller to deliver the goods for carriage as instructed by the buyer.

The “C”-terms require the seller to contract for carriage on usual terms at his own expense. Therefore, a point up to which he would have to pay transport costs must necessarily be indicated after the respective “C”-term.

The “D”-terms require the seller to be responsible for the arrival of the goods at the agreed place or point of destination at the border or within the country of import.

1.5 Module 5

“Ex works” means that the seller delivers when he places the goods at the disposal of the buyer at the seller’s premises or another named place (i.e., works, factory, warehouse) not cleared for export and not loaded on any collecting vehicle. This term thus represents the minimum obligation for the seller, and the buyer has to bear all costs and risks involved in taking the goods from the seller’s premises. However, if the parties wish the seller to be responsible for the loading of the goods on departure and to bear the risks and all the costs of such loading, this should be made clear by adding explicit wording to this effect in the contract of sale.

The seller must provide the goods and the commercial invoice, or its equivalent electronic message, in conformity with the contract of sale and any other evidence of conformity, which may be required by the contract. The seller must render the buyer, at the latter’s request, risk and expense, every assistance in obtaining, where applicable, any export license, or other official authorization necessary for the export of the goods. The buyer must pay the price as provided in the contract of sale. The buyer must bear all risks of loss of or damage to the goods from the time they have been delivered in accordance with the term’s delivery; and from the agreed date or the expiry date of any period fixed for taking delivery which arise because he fails to give notice in accordance with the notice of the seller, provided.

1.6 Module 6

“Free Carrier” means that the seller delivers the goods, cleared for export, to the carrier nominated by the buyer at the named place. It should be noted that the chosen place of delivery has an impact on the obligations of loading and unloading the goods at that place. If delivery occurs at the seller’s premises, the seller is responsible for loading. If delivery occurs at any other place, the seller is not responsible for unloading. This term may be used irrespective of the mode of transport, including multimodal transport.

“Carrier” means any person who, in a contract of carriage, undertakes to perform or to procure the performance of transport by rail, road, air, sea, inland waterway or by a combination of such modes. If the buyer nominates a person other than a carrier to receive the goods, the seller is deemed to have fulfilled his obligation to deliver the goods when they are delivered to that person.

The delivery is completed: (a) if the named place is the seller’s premises, when the goods have been loaded on the means of transport provided by the carrier nominated by the buyer or another person acting on his behalf; (b) if the named place is anywhere other than (a), when the goods are placed at the disposal of the carrier or another person nominated by the buyer, or chosen by the seller in accordance with the contract of carriage, on the seller’s means of transport not unloaded.

If no specific point has been agreed within the named place, and if there are several points available, the seller may select the point at the place of delivery which best suits his purpose.

Appendix 2: Model and data validation results

Variance inflation factors (VIFs) are reported in Table 3. These VIFs were calculated simultaneously for all variables, based on a full collinearity test (Charoensukmongkol 2014; Daniel and Chatelain-Jardon 2013). This test enables the identification of both vertical and lateral collinearity, the latter being collinearity among predictor–criterion variable pairs, and also of common method bias (Daniel and Chatelain-Jardon 2013; Kock and Lynn 2012). Stone-Geisser Q 2 coefficients (Geisser 1974; Stone 1974) are also shown for each of the six endogenous variables in the model, storing scores on learning tests for modules (M1M6). Stone-Geisser Q 2 coefficients are used for predictive validity assessment (Rejikumar 2013; Kock 2015a).

Table 3 Full collinearity VIFs and Q 2 coefficients

The absence of full collinearity VIFs > 3.3 is an indication that the model is free of both multicollinearity and common method bias (Daniel and Chatelain-Jardon 2013; Kock and Lynn 2012; Lindell and Whitney 2001), which as we can see is the case with our model. The absence of negative Q 2 coefficients is an indication of acceptable predictive validity in connection with all endogenous variables (Geisser 1974; Rejikumar 2013; Stone 1974; Kock 2015a), which is also the case with our model. Testing predictive validity is important here because all variables in our model are measured through single indicators, which precludes the use of other common validity tests in the context of confirmatory factor analyses (Thompson 2004), such as convergent and discriminant validity tests. Single-indicator measurement is a characteristic of the path analysis method, which differentiates it from structural equation modeling (Bowen and Guo 2011; Kline 2010).

Table 4 shows the skewness and excess kurtosis for all variables, as well as the results of two tests of normality: the classic Jarque–Bera test (Jarque and Bera 1980; Bera and Jarque 1981) and Gel and Gastwirth’s (2008) robust modification of this test. The results of the normality tests are shown in the columns labeled “Normal (Jarque–Bera)?” and “Normal (robust Jarque–Bera)?” The entry “Yes” indicates that the variable is normally distributed according to the test, and “No” that it is not normally distributed. As we can see, the results of both normality tests suggest that most of the variables were not normality distributed, supporting our decision to employ robust path analysis to test our hypotheses.

Table 4 Skewness, excess kurtosis, and normality tests

Living in a country does not necessarily mean that an individual is representative of that country’s national culture. Because of this, in studies including participants from different countries, it is important to assess the extent to which the participants “belong” to a national culture. This can be done through what is generally known as a manipulation check (Gürhan-Canli and Maheswaran 2000; Maheswaran 1994).

We conducted a manipulation check to assess whether the participants were representative of the countries they lived in at the time of the study. In order to do so, we asked questions related to two dimensions of Hofstede’s (2001) cultural dimensions model for which the USA and Mexico were markedly different, namely the power distance and masculinity/femininity dimensions. We then compared the means of the answers, provided on Likert-type scales going from 1 to 5, and found statistically significant differences consistent with the differences in Hofstede’s (2001) model for both power distance (β = .213, P < .001) and masculinity/femininity (β = .118, P = .01). This suggests that the participants were representative of the countries they lived in at the time of the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kock, N., Chatelain-Jardón, R. Surprise-enhanced and technology-mediated learning: a two-country study. Cogn Tech Work 18, 105–119 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-015-0349-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-015-0349-8

Keywords

Navigation