Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Cognitive bias and unusual experiences in childhood

  • Original Contribution
  • Published:
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cognitive therapy is recommended for children with psychotic-like, or unusual, experiences associated with distress or impairment (UEDs; UK National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2013 [1]). Accurate models of the psychological underpinnings of childhood UEDs are required to effectively target therapies. Cognitive biases, such as the jumping to conclusions data-gathering bias (JTC), are implicated in the development and maintenance of psychosis in adults. In this study, we aimed to establish the suitability for children of a task developed to assess JTC in adults. Eighty-six participants (aged 5–14 years) were recruited from Child and Adolescent Mental Health Service (CAMHS) and community (school) settings, and completed the probabilistic reasoning (‘Beads’) task, alongside measures of intellectual functioning, general psychopathology, and UEDs. Self-reported reasoning strategy was coded as ‘probabilistic’ or ‘other’. Younger children (5–10 years) were more likely than older children (11–14 years) to JTC (OR = 2.7, 95 % CI = 1.1–6.5, p = 0.03); and to use non-probabilistic reasoning strategies (OR = 9.4, 95 % CI = 1.7–48.8, p = 0.008). Both UED presence (OR = 5.1, 95 % CI = 1.2–21.9, p = 0.03) and lower IQ (OR = 0.9, 95 % CI = 0.9–1.0, p = 0.02) were significantly and independently associated with JTC, irrespective of age and task comprehension. Findings replicate research in adults, indicating that the ‘Beads’ task can be reliably employed in children to assess cognitive biases. Psychological treatments for children with distressing unusual experiences might usefully incorporate reasoning interventions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (2013) Psychosis and schizophrenia in children and young people. Recognition and management (CG155). National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, London

    Google Scholar 

  2. Linscott R, Van Os J (2012) An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychol Med 1:1–17

    Google Scholar 

  3. van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L (2009) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med 39:179–195

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Kelleher I, Connor D, Clarke MC, Devlin N, Harley M, Cannon M (2012) Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based studies. Psychol Med 42:1857–1863

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Laurens KR, Hobbs MJ, Sunderland M, Green MJ, Mould GL (2012) Psychotic-like experiences in a community sample of 8000 children aged 9 to 11 years: an item response theory analysis. Psychol Med 42:1495–1506

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Addington J, Cornblatt BA, Cadenhead KS, Cannon TD, McGlashan TH, Perkins DO, Seidman LJ, Tsuang MT, Walker EF, Woods SW (2011) At clinical high risk for psychosis: outcome for nonconverters. Am J Psychiatry 168:800

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Laurens KR, Hodgins S, Taylor EA, Murray RM (2011) Is earlier intervention for schizophrenia possible? Identifying antecedents of schizophrenia in children aged 9–12 years. In: David AS, Kapur S, McGuffin P (eds) Schizophrenia: the final frontier. Psychology Press, London, pp 19–32

    Google Scholar 

  8. Thapar A, Heron J, Jones RB, Owen MJ, Lewis G, Zammit S (2012) Trajectories of change in self-reported psychotic-like experiences in childhood and adolescence. Schizophr Res 140:104–109

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Asher L, Zammit S, Sullivan S, Dorrington S, Heron J, Lewis G (2013) The relationship between psychotic symptoms and social functioning in a non-clinical population of 12 year olds. Schizophr Res 150:404–409

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Ames CS, Jolley S, Laurens KR, Maddox L, Corrigall R, Browning S, Hirsch CR, Hassanali N, Bracegirdle K, Kuipers E (2014) Modeling psychosocial influences on the distress and impairment caused by psychotic like experiences in children and adolescents. Eur J Child Adolesc Psychiatry 23:715–722

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Kelleher I, Devlin N, Wigman JT, Kehoe A, Murtagh A, Fitzpatrick C, Cannon M (2014) Psychotic experiences in a mental health clinic sample: implications for suicidality, multimorbidity and functioning. Psychol Med 44:1615–1624

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Wigman JT, van Winkel R, Raaijmakers QA, Ormel J, Verhulst FC, Reijneveld SA, van Os J, Vollebergh WA (2011) Evidence for a persistent, environment-dependent and deteriorating subtype of subclinical psychotic experiences: a 6-year longitudinal general population study. Psychol Med 41:2317–2329

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Garety PA, Bebbington P, Fowler D, Freeman D, Kuipers E (2007) Implications for neurobiological research of cognitive models of psychosis: a theoretical paper. Psychol Med 37:1377–1391

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Garety P, Freeman D, Jolley S, Ross K, Waller H, Dunn G (2011) Jumping to conclusions: the psychology of delusional reasoning. Adv Psychiatr Treat 17:332–339

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Garety PA, Freeman D (2013) The past and future of delusions research: from the inexplicable to the treatable. Br J Psychiatry 203:327–333

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Fine C, Gardner M, Craigie J, Gold I (2007) Hopping, skipping or jumping to conclusions? Clarifying the role of the JTC bias in delusions. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 12:46–77

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Freeman D (2007) Suspicious minds: the psychology of persecutory delusions. Clin Psychol Rev 27:425–457

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Van Dael F, Versmissen D, Janssen I, Myin-Germeys I, van Os J, Krabbendam L (2006) Data gathering: biased in psychosis? Schizophr Bull 32:341–351

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Broome MR, Johns LC, Valli I, Woolley JB, Tabraham P, Brett C, Valmaggia L, Peters E, Garety PA, McGuire PK (2007) Delusion formation and reasoning biases in those at clinical high risk for psychosis. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 51:s38–s42

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Warman DM, Lysaker PH, Martin JM, Davis L, Haudenschield SL (2007) Jumping to conclusions and the continuum of delusional beliefs. Behav Res Ther 45:1255–1269

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Moritz S, Woodward TS, Lambert M (2007) Under what circumstances do patients with schizophrenia jump to conclusions? A liberal acceptance account. Br J Clin Psychol 46:127–137

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Colbert SM, Peters ER (2002) Need for closure and jumping-to-conclusions in delusion-prone individuals. J Nerv Ment Dis 190:27–31

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. McKay R, Langdon R, Coltheart M (2006) Need for closure, jumping to conclusions, and decisiveness in delusion-prone individuals. J Nerv Ment Dis 194:422–426

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. So S, Freeman D, Garety P (2008) A test of the impact of state anxiety on the jumping to conclusions delusion bias. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 42:879–886

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Dudley R, Shaftoe D, Cavanagh K, Spencer H, Ormrod J, Turkington D, Freeston M (2011) Jumping to conclusions’ in first-episode psychosis. Early Interv Psychiatry 5:50–56

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Langdon R, Still M, Connors MH, Ward PB, Catts SV (2014) Jumping to delusions in early psychosis. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 19:241–256

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Ross K, Freeman D, Dunn G, Garety P (2011) A randomized experimental investigation of reasoning training for people with delusions. Schizophr Bull 37:324–333

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Waller H, Freeman D, Jolley S, Dunn G, Garety P (2011) Targeting reasoning biases in delusions: a pilot study of the Maudsley Review Training Programme for individuals with persistent, high conviction delusions. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 42:414–421

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Moritz S, Veckenstedt R, Bohn F, Hottenrott B, Scheu F, Randjbar S, Aghotor J, Köther U, Woodward TS, Treszl A (2013) Complementary group Metacognitive Training (MCT) reduces delusional ideation in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 151:61–69

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Warman DM, Martin JM, Lysaker P (2013) Jumping to conclusions and delusions: the impact of discussion of the bias on the bias. Schizophr Res 150:575–579

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Garety P, Waller H, Emsley R, Jolley S, Kuipers E, Bebbington P, Dunn G, Fowler D, Hardy A, Freeman D (2014) Cognitive mechanisms of change in delusions: an experimental investigation targeting reasoning to effect change in Paranoia. Schizophr Bull. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu103

  32. van Oosterhout B, Krabbendam L, de Boer K, Ferwerda J, van der Helm M, Stant AD, van der Gaag M (2014) Metacognitive group training for schizophrenia spectrum patients with delusions: a randomized controlled trial. Psychol Med 44:3025–3035

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Favrod J, Rexhaj S, Bardy S, Ferrari P, Hayoz C, Moritz S, Conus P, Bonsack C (2014) Sustained antipsychotic effect of metacognitive training in psychosis: a randomized-controlled study. Eur Psychiatry 29:275–281

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Moritz S, Veckenstedt R, Andreou C, Bohn F, Hottenrott B, Leighton L, Köther U, Woodward TS, Treszl A, Menon M, Schneider BC, Pfueller U, Roesch-Ely D (2014) Sustained and “sleeper” effects of group metacognitive training for schizophrenia: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1038

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Thompson A, Sullivan S, Lewis G, Zammit S, Heron J, Horwood J, Thomas K, Gunnell D, Hollis C, Wolke D (2011) Association between locus of control in childhood and psychotic symptoms in early adolescence: results from a large birth cohort. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 16:385–402

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Fisher HL, Caspi A, Poulton R, Meier MH, Houts R, Harrington H, Arseneault L, Moffitt TE (2013) Specificity of childhood psychotic symptoms for predicting schizophrenia by 38 years of age: a birth cohort study. Psychol Med 43:2077–2086

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Fisher R (1998) Thinking about thinking: developing metacognition in children. Early Child Dev Care 141:1–15

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Schlottmann A, Wilkening F (2011) Judgment and decision making in young children. In: Dhami M, Schlottmann A, Waldmann M (eds) Judgment and decision-making as a skill: learning, development, evolution. Cambridge University, Cambridge, pp 55–83

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  39. Denison S, Bonawitz E, Gopnik A, Griffiths TL (2013) Rational variability in children’s causal inferences: the sampling hypothesis. Cognition 126:285–300

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Balzan R, Delfabbro P, Galletly C (2012) Delusion-proneness or miscomprehension? A re-examination of the jumping-to-conclusions bias. Aust J Psychol 64:100–107

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Falcone MA, Murray RM, Wiffen BD, O’Connor JA, Russo M, Kolliakou A, Stilo S, Taylor H, Gardner-Sood P, Paparelli A, Jichi F, Di Forti M, David AS, Freeman D, Jolley S (2014) Jumping to conclusions, neuropsychological functioning, and delusional beliefs in first episode psychosis. Schizophr Bull. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu104

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Dunn LM, Dunn LM, Whetton C, Burley J (1997) British picture vocabulary scale, 2nd edn. NFER-Nelson, Windsor

    Google Scholar 

  43. Garety PA, Freeman D, Jolley S, Dunn G, Bebbington PE, Fowler DG, Kuipers E, Dudley R (2005) Reasoning, emotions, and delusional conviction in psychosis. J Abnorm Psychol 114:373–384

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. So SH, Freeman D, Dunn G, Kapur S, Kuipers E, Bebbington P, Fowler D, Garety PA (2012) Jumping to conclusions, a lack of belief flexibility and delusional conviction in psychosis: a longitudinal investigation of the structure, frequency, and relatedness of reasoning biases. J Abnorm Psychol 121:129–139

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Laurens KR, Hodgins S, Maughan B, Murray RM, Rutter ML, Taylor EA (2007) Community screening for psychotic-like experiences and other putative antecedents of schizophrenia in children aged 9–12 years. Schizophr Res 90:130–146

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Costello A, Edelbrock C, Kalas R, Kessler M, Klaric S (1982) Diagnostic interview for children: child version. National Institute of Mental Health, Rockville

    Google Scholar 

  47. Goodman R (2001) Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 40:1337–1345

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Cohen J (1992) A power primer. Psychol Bull 112:155–159

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Chen H, Cohen P, Chen S (2010) How big is a big odds ratio? Interpreting the magnitudes of odds ratios in epidemiological studies. Commun Stat Simul Comput® 39:860–864

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Moritz S, Van Quaquebeke N, Lincoln TM (2012) Jumping to conclusions is associated with paranoia but not general suspiciousness: a comparison of two versions of the probabilistic reasoning paradigm. Schizophr Res Treat 2012:384039. doi:10.1155/2012/384039

  51. Garety PA, Gittins M, Jolley S, Bebbington P, Dunn G, Kuipers E, Fowler D, Freeman D (2013) Differences in cognitive and emotional processes between persecutory and grandiose delusions. Schizophr Bull 39:629–639

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all the children who participated in this research and their parental responsibility holders, together with the staff of the participating school and clinic. The authors gratefully acknowledge financial support for the work through a grant from the Guy’s & St Thomas’ Charity (ref. R100417). EK, KRL and CH are partly supported by and/or are affiliated with, the National Institute for Health Research Mental Health Biomedical Research Centre and Dementia Unit (BRC/U) at the South London and Maudsley National Health Service Foundation Trust and Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, United Kingdom. KRL was supported in part by funding from a National Institute of Health Research Career Development Fellowship (CDF/08/01/015).

Conflict of Interest

There were no conflicts of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Suzanne Jolley.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hassanali, N., Ruffell, T., Browning, S. et al. Cognitive bias and unusual experiences in childhood. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 24, 949–957 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-014-0644-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-014-0644-6

Keywords

Navigation