Introduction

Postpartum anxiety (PPA) remains among the most under-studied, under-diagnosed, and under-treated complications of childbirth (Smith and Kipnis 2012). Moreover, PPA has been associated with a variety of suboptimal outcomes in the infant (Lonstein 2007; Glasheen et al. 2010). Research into PPA currently utilises a range of general self-report measures which are seldom validated for use postpartum. This may lead to erroneous data, inaccurate interpretation, and incomparable results across studies (Meades and Ayers 2011).

These oversights have been addressed in the pregnancy anxiety literature (Van den Bergh 1990; Levin 1991; Wadwha et al. 1993; Huizink et al. 2002) where a distinct presentation from general anxiety and depression (Huizink et al. 2004) has been revealed. As a result, a number of self-report measures have been developed (e.g. Pregnancy Related Anxiety Questionnaire (PRAQ; Van Den Bergh 1990), and the PRAQ-R (Huizink et al. 2004)). Studies using these scales consistently find that pregnancy-specific anxiety is a more efficacious predictor of perinatal outcomes than other general forms of stress, anxiety, and depression (Guardino and Schetter 2014). This is evident across studies of pre-term birth (Dunkel Schetter 2011), cognitive and motor performance (Davis and Sandman 2010), attention regulation (Huizink et al. 2002), temperament (Davis et al. 2004), and infant-feeding (Fairlee et al. 2009).

An equivalent measure of postpartum-specific anxiety has recently been developed and validated (Fallon et al. 2016b). The Postpartum-specific Anxiety Scale (PSAS) is a 51-item scale that taps into four domains of anxiety which are specific to the postpartum period. The PSAS proved acceptable to postpartum women and demonstrated high validity and reliability in initial psychometric work. As with all novel measures, validation is an iterative process and the predictive utility of the PSAS has not yet been examined in comparison to its pregnancy-specific counterparts.

One fundamental infant health outcome lies in the nourishment of the infant. Appropriate infant-feeding (i.e. responsive maternal feeding, exclusive breastfeeding to 6 months of age) confers significant health benefits. A recent systematic review provides evidence that women with PPA are less likely to breastfeed exclusively, and more likely to terminate breastfeeding earlier (Fallon et al. 2016a). Furthermore, mothers who report anxiety are at risk of non-responsive feeding behaviours (Hurley et al. 2008) characterised by impaired feeding interactions, insensitivity to infant cues of hunger and satiety, and lack of uptake to current feeding recommendations (Birch and Fisher 1995; Hughes et al. 2005).

The aims of this paper are twofold. Firstly, the predictive validity of the PSAS will be examined within the context of infant-feeding using a short-term prospective design. Second, it will be examined whether the PSAS may be more efficacious at predicting infant-feeding and perceptions of infant-feeding behaviours than the more commonly used general measures. It is hypothesised that after controlling for the effects of general anxiety and depression, postpartum-specific anxiety will have a significant, independent effect on infant-feeding outcomes and perceptions of infant-feeding behaviours.

Methods

Participants

Mothers of infants aged between birth and 6 months postpartum were recruited via online advertising techniques providing a link to the Qualtrics survey software. A self-selecting subsample returned to complete the follow-up questionnaire 2 weeks later.

Design and procedure

A short-term online prospective design was utilised. The main questionnaire comprised the maternal mental health measures (i.e. PSAS, STAI-S, STAI-T, BDI-II) in addition to demographic variables. This was accessible from 4 September 2015 to 5 November 2015.

Participants were then asked if they would like to return 2 weeks later to complete a follow-up survey. Those who were willing received an e-mail with the follow-up survey (BEBQ, feeding outcome items) exactly 2 weeks later. The link to the follow-up questionnaire was only active on the day it was distributed. Participants completing both questionnaires received a reimbursement of £10.

Measures

Demographics

Maternal and infant demographic questions were asked at the beginning of the main questionnaire (see Table 1). Maternal height and weight values were converted to metric units and a maternal BMI (kg/m2) variable was computed for analyses. Occupational prestige, educational attainment, size of household, and living status were combined to create a composite measure of socio-economic status (SES) for analyses. Infant weight and length values were converted to metric units and infant BMI z-scores and percentiles were calculated using weight, length, age, and gender information.

Table 1 Maternal and infant characteristics (N = 261)

The Postpartum-specific Anxiety Scale (PSAS; Fallon et al. 2016b)

The PSAS (see Fallon et al. 2016b for items and factor loadings) is designed to measure the frequency of maternal and infant-focused anxieties experienced during the past week. It contains 51 items across four distinct constructs specific to the first 6 months after birth. ‘Competence and attachment anxieties’ (15 items) addresses anxieties relating to maternal self-efficacy, parenting competence and the mother-infant relationship. ‘Safety and welfare anxieties’ (11 items) examines fears about infant illnesses, accidents, and cot death. ‘Practical baby care anxieties’ (7 items) covers anxieties that are specific to infant care such as feeding, sleeping, and general routine. ‘Psychosocial adjustment to motherhood’ (18 items) addresses postpartum adjustment concerns including management of personal appearance, relationships and support, work and finances, and sleep. The PSAS was found to be acceptable to postpartum women and performed well in reliability and validity analyses.

The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck et al. 1988)

The BDI is a commonly used self-report tool for detecting and measuring general depression. It contains 21 items designed to measure the severity of general depression experienced during the past 2 weeks. Higher scores indicate more severe depressive symptoms. Twenty-five years of psychometric testing provides evidence of its reliability and validity in clinical and non-clinical samples (Beck et al. 1988).

The Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger et al. 1970)

The STAI is a self-report measure designed to capture levels of general anxiety. It contains 40 items with two separate subscales (20 items each) to measure situational (state) and stable (trait) anxiety. Higher scores on each four-point Likert scale item indicate higher levels of anxiety. The STAI is a reliable and valid measure used with clinical and non-clinical populations and more recently in perinatal samples (Meades and Ayers 2011; Spielberger et al. 1970).

Infant-feeding outcomes

Two researcher-developed 7-point Likert-scale items were used to ascertain current feeding method and prenatal feeding intention. Available answers were based on WHO-defined categories (WHO 2002). Mothers were asked ‘How are you currently feeding your baby?’ and available response options were as follows: ‘exclusively breastfeeding (100%)’, ‘predominately breast milk (over 80%) with a little formula (under 20%)’, ‘mainly breast milk (50%–80%) with some formula’, ‘a combination of both breast milk (50%) and formula (50%)’, ‘mainly formula (50%–80%) with some breast milk’, ‘predominately formula (over 80%) with a little breast milk (under 20%)’, and ‘exclusively formula feeding (100%)’. Mothers were then asked ‘How were you planning to feed your baby in pregnancy’, and the same response options were provided.

Baby Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (BEBQ; Llewellyn et al. 2011)

The BEBQ is a 17-item parental-report measure of infant-feeding behaviour during the period of exclusive milk feeding. It comprises four distinct feeding traits and one item describing general appetite. ‘Enjoyment of food’ (4 items) describes the infant’s perceived liking of milk and of feeding in general. ‘Food responsiveness’ (6 items) relates to how demanding the infant is with regard to being fed and his or her level of responsiveness to cues of milk and feeding. ‘Slowness in eating’ (4 items) evaluates the speed with which an infant typically feeds, and ‘satiety responsiveness’ (3 items) examines how easily the infant gets full during a feed. The item ‘My baby has a big appetite’ correlated with all scales and can be used as an individual item to measure overall appetite. The BEBQ demonstrated good reliability and validity in initial psychometric testing (Llewellyn et al. 2011).

Method of analysis

To develop a comprehensive model, a range of potentially confounding variables was identified from previous literature (see Table 1). Bivariate analyses were conducted between each potential confounder, the exposure of interest (i.e. PSAS scores), and the outcome of interest (i.e. feeding outcome or behaviour). Confounders significantly associated with both exposure and outcome at 10% level were included in the final regression models.

The current feeding method categories (N = 5) were collapsed into two binary variables: (‘exclusively breast feeding’ yes/no, and ‘any breastfeeding’ yes/no). Concurrently, the initial feeding intention categories were also collapsed (‘exclusive breastfeeding intention’ yes/no, and ‘any breastfeeding intention’ yes/no). Two hierarchical binary logistic regressions (HLRs) were conducted to analyse the effect of PSAS scores in the main questionnaire on infant-feeding outcomes in the follow-up questionnaire. Relevant confounders were entered in block 1, followed by general measures of anxiety and depression in block 2. The PSAS was entered into the final block. Odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated to describe the predictive value of each variable.

Using the same entry method, a hierarchical linear multiple regression analysis (HMR) was conducted to analyse the effect of PSAS scores in the main questionnaire on perceptions of infant-feeding behaviours at follow-up. β and p values were calculated to describe the predictive value of each variable. Variance inflation factors (VIFs) were > 5 for the general measures of anxiety and depression in block 2 which warrants concern. The three measures (STAI-S, STAI-T, BDI) were converted to z-scores and combined, and the regression was conducted again with the composite variable. Results (R 2, β, and p values) were analogous, so the original entry method was used to provide the most informative output.

Results

Participants

Of the 1282 recruited, a total of 800 (62%) completed the main questionnaire. Of these, 261 returned to complete the follow-up questionnaire (33%). Among those completing both surveys, maternal age ranged from 19 to 44 years (M = 31.25; SD = 4.50). The sample were predominately married (75%), primiparous (46%), and housewives (44%) from the UK (92%). Twenty-seven (10%) of the women had a current, clinical diagnosis of anxiety or depression which is comparable with UK prevalence estimates. The infant age ranged from 1 to 26 weeks (M = 16.10; SD = 6.43). Sixty-seven percent of the infants were exclusively breastfed, and 83% of the infants were receiving breastmilk in any quantity. See Table 1 for demographic details. There were no difference in mean scores on any of the mood measures between those completing both surveys and those completing only the first survey (PSAS: t = 0.86, p = .39; STAI-S: t = 1.28, p = .20; STAI-T: t = 1.30, p = .19; BDI: t = 0.02, p = .99). Mothers completing both surveys did not differ from those completing only the first survey with respect to age, marital status, and BMI. However, mothers completing both surveys were more likely to have higher SES scores (20.85 ± 3.33 vs 20.23 ± 3.59; t = −2.38, p = .02) and less likely to have a current, clinical diagnosis of anxiety or depression (22.1 vs 77.9%, χ 2 = 4.57, p = .03). Infants of mothers completing both surveys did not differ from those only completing the first survey on any characteristic.

HLR predicting exclusive breastfeeding status (Table 2)

The final regression model significantly predicted exclusive breastfeeding status, correctly identifying 79.9% of cases: Cox and Snell R 2 = .24, Nagelkerke R 2 = .33, p < .001. The covariates in step 1 explained approximately 20% (Cox and Snell) and 29% (Nagelkerke) of the variance in exclusive breastfeeding. General measures of anxiety and depression (step 2) explained approximately 2% (Cox and Snell) and 2% (Nagelkerke) of the variance but were not significant predictors of exclusive breastfeeding. However, in the final step, the PSAS was a significant predictor of exclusive breastfeeding which explained approximately 2% (Cox and Snell) and 2% (Nagelkerke) of the variance. Higher PSAS scores were associated with lower odds of exclusive breastfeeding (OR 0.98; CI 0.96, 0.97).

HLR predicting any breastfeeding status (Table 2)

The final regression model significantly predicted any breastfeeding status, correctly identifying 85.6% of cases: Cox and Snell R 2 = .17, Nagelkerke R 2 = .29, p < .001. The covariates in step 1 explained approximately 14% (Cox and Snell) and 23% (Nagelkerke) of the variance in any breastfeeding. General measures of anxiety and depression (step 2) explained approximately 2% (Cox and Snell) and 3% (Nagelkerke) of the variance but were not significant predictors of any breastfeeding. However, in the final step, the PSAS was a significant predictor of exclusive breastfeeding which explained approximately 1% (Cox and Snell) and 3% (Nagelkerke) of the variance. Higher PSAS scores were associated with lower odds of any breastfeeding (OR 0.97; CI 0.95, 0.99).

Table 2 Hierarchical logistic regression demonstrating postpartum specific anxiety as a predictor of exclusive breastfeeding status and any breastfeeding status after controlling for general measures of mood

HMR predicting infant enjoyment of food (Table 3)

The final regression model predicted approximately 21% of the variance in general appetite scores (R 2 = .21, F (7,252) = 9.28, p < .001). The covariates in step 1 explained approximately 8% of the variance in enjoyment of food but were not significant predictors. General anxiety and depression (step 2) explained approximately 9% of the variance; again, these predictors were not significant. However, in the final step, the PSAS was a highly significant predictor which explained approximately 4% variance in enjoyment of food. Higher PSAS scores were associated with lower perceived enjoyment of food in the infant (β = − 0.33; p < .001).

Table 3 Hierarchical regression analysis demonstrating postpartum-specific anxiety as a predictor of infant enjoyment of food after controlling for general measures of mood

HMR predicting infant food responsiveness (Table 4)

The final regression model predicted approximately 27% of the variance in food responsiveness scores (R 2 = .27, F (8,251) = 11.71, p < .001). The covariates in step 1 explained approximately 12% of the variance in food responsiveness. General anxiety and depression scores (step 2) explained approximately 9% of the variance in scores, although these predictors were not significant. In the final step, the PSAS was a highly significant predictor which explained approximately 6% of the variance in food responsiveness scores. Higher PSAS scores were associated with greater perceived food responsiveness in the infant (β = 0.43; p < .001).

Table 4 Hierarchical regression analysis demonstrating postpartum specific anxiety as a predictor of infant food responsiveness after controlling for general measures of mood

HMR predicting infant satiety responsiveness (Table 5)

The final regression model predicted approximately 10% of the variance in satiety responsiveness scores (R 2 = 0.10, F (8,251) = 3.65, p < .001). The covariates in step 1 explained approximately 7% of the variance. General anxiety and depression (step 2) explained approximately 2% of the variance in scores; only general depression was significant and negatively associated with the outcome (β = − 0.29, p = .047). In the final step, the PSAS was also a significant predictor which explained approximately 6% of the variance in satiety responsiveness scores. However, PSAS scores were positively associated with perceptions of satiety responsiveness (β =  .24; p = .03).

Table 5 Hierarchical regression analysis demonstrating postpartum-specific anxiety as a predictor of infant satiety responsiveness after controlling for general measures of mood

HMR predicting infant slowness in eating (Table 6)

The final regression model predicted approximately 8% of the variance in slowness of eating scores (R 2 = .08, F (7,252) = 3.01, p = .005). The covariates in step 1 explained approximately 6% of the variance. General anxiety and depression (step 2) explained approximately 1% of the variance in scores and were not significant. In the final step, the PSAS explained approximately 1% of the variance and was not a significant predictor of slowness in eating.

Table 6 Hierarchical regression analysis demonstrating postpartum specific anxiety as a predictor of infant slowness in eating after controlling for general measures of mood

HMR predicting infant general appetite (Table 7)

The final regression model predicted approximately 10% of the variance in general appetite scores (R 2 = .10, F (8,251) = 3.27, p = .001). The covariates in step 1 explained approximately 9% of the variance in general appetite. General anxiety and depression scores (step 2) explained approximately 1% of the variance, although these predictors were not significant. In the final step, PSAS scores explained no variance and were not a significant predictor of general appetite.

Table 7 Hierarchical regression analysis showing postpartum specific anxiety as a predictor of infant general appetite after controlling for general measures of mood

Discussion

Results demonstrate that higher levels of postpartum-specific anxiety are associated with lower odds of breastfeeding exclusively, and breastfeeding in any quantity in the first 6 months postpartum. These findings provide evidence for the predictive validity of the PSAS as they are comparable with a body of literature which finds that PPA is inversely associated with exclusive breastfeeding (Virden 1988; Groër 2005; Clifford 2006; Britton 2007; Zanardo et al. 2009; Adedinsewo et al. 2014) and breastfeeding in any quantity (Adedinsewo et al. 2014; Britton 2007; Brown and Arnott 2014; Courtois et al. 2014; Paul et al. 2013). Biological theories of anxiety and lactation posit that PPA may negatively influence breastfeeding through physiological stress responses and subsequent hormone imbalance (Lonstein 2007). In particular, anxiety is associated with lower oxytocin and prolactin which may inhibit the milk ejection reflex and subsequent breast milk production (Chen et al. 1998; Dewey 2001; Stuebe et al. 2012). It is theorised that postpartum-specific anxieties negatively affect breastfeeding practices via the same physiological mechanisms.

The current study also observed that postpartum-specific anxiety was significantly associated with negative perceptions of infant-feeding behaviours including a lower enjoyment of food, and greater food responsiveness and satiety responsiveness in the infant. The latter two findings may initially appear counterintuitive; food responsiveness is characterised by excessive hungriness, unnecessary and frequent demands for milk, and an inability to recognise satiety cues. Conversely, satiety responsiveness is characterised by under-consumption of milk during feeds and an oversensitivity to cues of satiety (Llewellyn et al. 2011). However, all three findings map onto previous work which finds high levels of anxiety negatively affect maternal perceptions of infant-feeding behaviour and impede maternal ability to interpret infant cues of hunger and satiety (Farrow and Blissett 2005; Hellin and Waller 1992; Hurley et al. 2008; Richter and Reck 2013; Savage et al. 2008). This study adds to this by demonstrating that postpartum-specific anxiety can negatively affect maternal perceptions across a number of feeding domains simultaneously and regardless of polarity. Distorted perceptions are a fundamental characteristic of unresponsive feeding practices which are linked to a range of adverse health outcomes (Cullen et al. 2000; Farrow and Blissett 2008; Hurley et al. 2011). Despite these findings, postpartum-specific anxiety was not associated with slowness in eating or general appetite. Given that this was the case for all of the measures of mood in these models it may be that these domains of feeding behaviour are too diffuse or that they do not elicit the same emotional response from mothers.

As hypothesised, postpartum-specific anxiety was a stronger predictor of infant-feeding outcomes and behaviours than general anxiety (state and trait) and depression. The PSAS was the only significant measure of mood across all of the feeding outcomes and behaviours apart from slowness in eating and general appetite analyses (in which none of the measures of mood were significant). Furthermore, the PSAS was a significant predictor after controlling for general anxiety and depression indicating that postpartum-specific anxiety elicits a unique effect upon infant-feeding outcomes and behaviours. This is a novel finding in the postpartum anxiety literature and resonates with a body of work which finds pregnancy-specific anxiety is a more potent predictor across a range of infant health and behaviour outcomes (Davis and Sandman 2010; Dunkel Schetter 2011; Guardino and Schetter 2014; Huizink et al. 2002, 2003). Theories of pregnancy-specific anxiety posit that it is a distinct construct which is rooted in the emotional and physical context of a specific pregnancy (Huizink et al. 2004). It has been suggested that pregnancy-specific anxiety may differ in its predictive power because it is more proximally linked to physiological stress responses than general measures of mood (Guardino and Schetter 2014). Furthermore, studies attempting to distinguish between general and pregnancy-specific anxiety typically report moderate correlations, suggesting that there is overlap but there is also an inimitable construct (Rini et al. 1999; Green et al. 2003; Huizink et al. 2004). These findings extend the applicability of this theory to the postpartum period. It is proposed that postpartum-specific anxiety is a distinct construct which is embedded in the emotional and physical context of the months following childbirth with a new infant. The study provides new evidence for childbearing specific measures of mood in the postnatal period and calls for an increased uptake in the use of these measures when attempting to predict childbearing related outcomes. Future research should aim to replicate these findings across other indices of maternal and infant health and behaviour in the postpartum period, particularly those with previously inconsistent results using general measures.

One strength of this study is its simultaneous consideration of infant-feeding outcomes and behaviours which provides a more comprehensive overview of the relationship between PPA and infant-feeding than other work (Fallon et al. 2016a). Furthermore, the analysis distinguished between exclusive and any breastfeeding which is consistent with current breastfeeding recommendations (McAndrew et al. 2012) and more detailed than previous research (Dusdieker et al. 1985; Hellin and Waller 1992; Mezzacappa and Katkin 2002; Cooke et al. 2007; O’Brien et al. 2008; Courtois et al. 2014). However, some limitations must also be acknowledged. Although the study controlled for a range of established confounders, the short-term prospective design precludes causality. Future research should aim to replicate the findings prospectively over a longer follow-up period. An online convenience sample was used which was adequately powered for the analyses conducted but lacked sampling control. As such, the samples were predominately married, primiparous, housewives which limit the generalizability of findings to other populations. Finally, although the PSAS was a highly significant predictor in the models discussed, the variance explained in the outcome variables was low which indicates that there is a reliable, albeit small, relationship between variables. Infant-feeding practices are complex and multifaceted, with many cultural (Scott et al. 2015), social (Hauff 2014), physical (Arbour and Kessler 2013), and emotional (O’Brien et al. 2008) factors affecting behaviours and outcomes. Given the current lack of uptake to infant-feeding recommendations (McAndrew et al. 2012), identification of any factor that consistently impacts upon feeding practices is important.

The domains of anxiety encompassed in the PSAS may all be potentially modifiable through support, education, and treatment. Replication of these findings in relation to infant-feeding and other fundamental maternal and infant health outcomes will provide an evidence base to inform interventions aimed at reducing postpartum-specific anxiety. Interventions designed to alter feeding perceptions in anxious postpartum populations may also increase the likelihood of positive feeding interactions, reduce the onset of feeding difficulties, and alleviate the emotional consequences brought about by them.