Skip to main content
Log in

BOLD response during visual perception of biological motion in obsessive-compulsive disorder

An fMRI study using the dynamic point-light animation paradigm

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objective

Although research has shown that deficits in various cognitive functions may underlie obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), studies have not yet clarified the specificity and etiology of perception processing, particularly the perception of biological motion that is correlated with social cognition. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate neural activity associated with the perception of biological motion in OCD patients.

Methods

The subjects were 15 patients with OCD and 15 age- and IQ-matched healthy volunteers. All subjects participated in a biological motion task in which they performed a one-back task signaling a repeated stimulus with a key press in each block condition to obligate attention to both types of stimuli.

Results

The biological motion versus scrambled motion contrast revealed that both OCD patients and healthy controls exhibited increased activation of the superior and middle temporal gyrus, the regions implicated in processing of biological motion, which is consistent with previous studies. However, direct comparison between OCD subjects and healthy controls indicated that patients with OCD exhibited increased activation in the right superior and middle temporal gyrus and the left inferior temporal and fusiform gyrus, and reduced activation in the right postcentral gyrus (BA 40) compared to healthy subjects. OCD patients exhibited increased activation in the ventral visual system, including the inferior temporal and fusiform gyrus.

Discussion

We observed a differential pattern of activity between OCD patients and healthy controls, indicating that OCD patients have functional differences related to the perception of biological motion. The differential activation between OCD patients and healthy subjects might contribute to the pathophysiological understanding of obsessive compulsive disorder.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Adolphs R (1999) Social cognition and the human brain. Trends Cogn Sci 3:469–479

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Aggleton JP, Burton MJ, Passingham RE (1980) Cortical and subcortical afferents to the amygdala of the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). Brain Res 190:347–368

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Allison T, Puce A, McCarthy G (2000) Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn Sci 4:267–278

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Barbas H (1988) Anatomic organization of basoventral and mediodorsal visual recipient prefrontal regions in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 276:313–342

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Bullmore ET, Wheelwright S, Ashwin C, Williams SC (2000) The amygdala theory of autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 24:355–364

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Johansson G (1973) Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Percept Psychophys 14:201–211

    Google Scholar 

  7. Blake R, Shiffrar M (2007) Perception of human motion. Annu Rev Psychol 58:47–73

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Blake R, Turner LM, Smoski MJ, Pozdol SL, Stone WL (2003) Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychol Sci 14:151–157

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Buccino G, Binkofski F, Riggio L (2004) The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain Lang 89:370–376

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Calvo-Merino B, Grezes J, Glaser DE, Passingham RE, Haggard P (2006) Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Curr Biol 16:1905–1910

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Chen Y, Nakayama K, Levy D, Matthysse S, Holzman P (2003) Processing of global, but not local, motion perception is deficient in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 61:215–227

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Choi JS, Kim HS, Yoo SY, Ha TH, Chang JH, Kim YY, Shin YW, Kwon JS (2006) Morphometric alterations of anterior superior temporal cortex in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress Anxiety 23:290–296

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Cottraux J, Gerard D, Cinotti L, Froment JC, Deiber MP, Le Bars D, Galy G, Millet P, Labbé C, Lavenne F, Bouvard M, Mauguière F et al (1996) A controlled positron emission tomography study of obsessive and neutral auditory stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder with checking rituals. Psychiatry Res 60:101–112

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M (1997) Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV personality disorders (SCID-II). American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  15. Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolatti G (2002) Action representation and the inferior parietal lobule. In: Prinz W, Hommel B (eds) Attention and performance, vol XIX. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 334–355

    Google Scholar 

  16. Gallese V (2003) The roots of empathy: the shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology 36:171–180

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Giese MA, Poggio T (2003) Neural mechanisms for the recognition of biological movements. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:179–192

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Goodale MA (1997) Visual routes to perception and action in the cerebral cortex. In: Boller F (ed) Handbook of neuropsychology. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 91–109

    Google Scholar 

  19. Grisham JR, Anderson TM, Sachdev PS (2008) Genetic and environmental influences on obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 258:107–116

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Grossman E, Donnelly M, Price R, Pickens D, Morgan V, Neighbor G, Blake R (2000) Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. J Cogn Neurosci 12:711–720

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Grossman ED, Blake R (2002) Brain areas active during visual perception of biological motion. Neuron 35:1167–1175

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Heberlein AS, Saxe RR (2005) Dissociation between emotion and personality judgements: convergent evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuroimage 28:770–777

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Inoue Y, Yamada K, Hirano M, Shinohara M, Tamaoki T, Iguchi H, Tonooka Y, Kanba S (2006) Impairment of theory of mind in patients in remission following first episode of schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 256:326–328

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Kang DH, Kwon JS, Kim JJ, Youn T, Park HJ, Kim MS, Lee DS, Lee MC (2003) Brain glucose metabolic changes associated with neuropsychological improvements after 4 months of treatment in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 107:291–297

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Kim J, Blake R, Park S, Shin YW, Kang DH, Kwon JS (2007) Selective impairment in visual perception of biological motion in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress Anxiety (in press)

  26. Kim J, Doop ML, Blake R, Park S (2005) Impaired visual recognition of biological motion in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 77:299–307

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Loula F, Prasad S, Harber K, Shiffrar M (2005) Recognizing people from their movement. J Exp Psychol Human Percept Perform 31:210–220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Lucey JV, Costa DC, Blanes T, Busatto GF, Pilowsky LS, Takei N, Marks IM, Ell PJ, Kerwin RW (1995) Regional cerebral blood flow in obsessive-compulsive disordered patients at rest. Differential correlates with obsessive-compulsive and anxious-avoidant dimensions. Br J Psychiatry 167:629–634

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Mataix-Cols D, Rauch SL, Manzo PA, Jenike MA, Baer L (1999) Use of factor-analyzed symptom dimensions to predict outcome with serotonin reuptake inhibitors and placebo in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 156: 1409–1416

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Olley A, Malhi G, Sachdev P (2007) Memory and executive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a selective review. J Affect Disord 104:15–23

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Perkins DO, Lieberman J (2003) Implications for the neural basis of social cognition for the study of schizophrenia. Am J psychiatry 160:815–824

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V (2001) Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:661–670

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Saxe R, Xiao DK, Kovacs G, Perrett DI, Kanwisher N (2004) A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions. Neuropsychologia 42:1435–1446

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Saxena S, Bota RG, Brody AL (2001) Brain-behavior relationships in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry 6:82–101

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Saygin AP (2007) Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary for biological motion perception. Brain 130:2452–2461

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Shin MS, Park SJ, Kim MS, Lee YH, Ha TH, Kwon JS (2004) Deficits of organizational strategy and visual memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology 18:665–672

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Shin YW, Yoo SY, Lee JK, Ha TH, Lee KJ, Lee JM, Kim IY, Kim SI, Kwon JS (2007) Cortical thinning in obsessive compulsive disorder. Hum Brain Mapp 28:1128–1135

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Szeszko PR, Ardekani BA, Ashtari M, Malhotra AK, Robinson DG, Bilder RM, Lim KO (2005) White matter abnormalities in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62:782–790

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Watanabe K, Shimojo S (1998) Attentional modulation in perception of visual motion events. Perception 27:1041–1054

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Wheeler ME, Buckner RL (2003) Functional dissociation among components of remembering: control, perceived oldness, and content. J Neurosci 23:3869–3880

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Wiener D, Rybakowski J (2006) Disturbances of social cognition in schizophrenia. Psychiatr Pol 40:205–21

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant (M103KV010012-08K2201-01210) from the Brain Research Center of the 21st Century Frontier Research Program funded by the Ministry of Science and Technology, Republic of Korea. The authors thank Randolph Blake, Sohee Park and Jejoong Kim for developing the task of biological motion and their comments on this article. The authors also thank Yong-Sik Jung for MR imaging technical assistance and the staff of the Clinical Cognitive Neuroscience Center for help with recruitment.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jun Soo Kwon MD, PhD.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Jung, W.H., Gu, BM., Kang, DH. et al. BOLD response during visual perception of biological motion in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 259, 46–54 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-008-0833-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-008-0833-8

Keywords

Navigation