Introduction

Predation is a key factor in shaping ecological communities (Sih 1985), and the direct impact of apex carnivores goes beyond their primary prey species, extending to mesocarnivores, i.e., carnivores that are mid-ranking in a food web (Prugh et al. 2009). Apex carnivores can negatively affect mesocarnivores through direct predation, kleptoparasitism, and harassment (Prugh et al. 2009; Ritchie and Johnson 2009); yet, smaller carnivores do manage to coexist with numerous apex carnivores. For example, in Serengeti National Park in Tanzania where lions (Panthera leo) and spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta) live at high densities, there are eight species of mesocarnivores in the felid and canid families alone (Estes 1991). This raises a question about carnivore coexistence: what behaviors do mesocarnivores use to minimize negative interactions with dangerous larger predators? Illuminating coexistence strategies can expand our knowledge of how diverse communities of carnivores are structured and maintained (Vanak et al. 2013), and potentially aid in our understanding of how top-down pressures affect relationships between mesopredators and their prey (Dunphy-Daly et al. 2010; Suraci et al. 2016).

Many of the known strategies mesocarnivores use to minimize risk from apex predators rely on spatial avoidance. For example, wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) can coexist with lions by shifting their core areas to places lions do not use (Darnell et al. 2014). In the presence of wolves (Canis lupus) and coyote (Canis latrans), home ranges tend to occur in between, or on the edges of, wolf pack territories (Fuller and Keith 1981; Arjo and Pletscher 1999). However, in systems where apex predator densities are high and their habitat use is broad, avoiding them completely may not be possible, and more fine scale strategies are likely to come into play. For example, in both the Okavango Delta of Botswana and Serengeti National Park in Tanzania, cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) avoid lions and spotted hyenas on short temporal and spatial scales (Durant 1998, 2000a; Broekhuis et al. 2013; Swanson et al. 2016), which allows them to coexist within the larger landscape. However, fine-scale avoidance requires the ability to react appropriately and rapidly to changes in current risk, which can negatively affect foraging behavior. For example, the proximity of larger carnivores lowers the chances that cheetahs will initiate a hunt (Durant 1998, 2000a; Cooper et al. 2007). Once a hunt has begun, moving to avoid larger carnivores would involve abandoning a kill or losing opportunities to hunt prey, and therefore may not be the optimal reaction to short-term changes in risk. Thus, it is probable that mesocarnivores will choose less costly modifications of their foraging behavior in order to hunt and retain sufficient prey while avoiding potentially dangerous interactions with larger predators.

Foraging in the presence of predators is inherently risky, and the tradeoffs between time spent foraging and safety have been extensively studied (Brown 1988; Verdolin 2006). Vigilance is a common strategy used by a wide variety of taxa to lower predation risk (Bøving and Post 1997; Toïgo 1999; Randall and Boltas King 2001; Favreau et al. 2010). Mesocarnivores are no exception, for example, captured wild stoats (Mustela ermine) were more vigilant while feeding in patches closer to caged ferrets (Mustela furo) or feral cats (Felis catus) (Garvey et al. 2015); and coyotes scavenging carcasses in Yellowstone National Park became more vigilant once wolves were reintroduced (Switalski 2003). Larger mesocarnivores like cheetahs and wild dogs may face lower predation risk than smaller species, but since they hunt relatively large prey that cannot be consumed rapidly, they may increase the risk of losing their kills to apex predators (kleptoparasitism) (Gorman et al. 1998; Hunter et al. 2007a). One strategy to lessen kleptoparasitism is to spend less time with the carcass, lowering the chances of detection by predators. For example, wild dogs, who pay a steep metabolic cost when kills are stolen (Gorman et al. 1998), eat the majority of the carcass within 15 min (Carbone et al. 2005).

This range of responses by mesocarnivores to different threats from larger predators suggests that the strategies used to minimize risk while handling prey could follow a continuum. At one end are behaviors that maximize amount of food consumed before the kill is potentially stolen. However, eating quickly usually requires spending extended periods with a lowered head, which is risky since it prevents scanning for incoming threats. A larger predator could approach a feeding mesocarnivore undetected, creating the possibility of a dangerous encounter. Therefore when predation is the primary concern and an encounter is potentially extremely dangerous, we would expect to see behaviors from the other end of the continuum, i.e., behaviors that prioritize vigilance and safety over speed in eating.

Not all individuals are equally vulnerable to predation (Pettorelli et al. 2011), and their reactions to threats from predators may vary as well. To examine whether individual cheetahs use different prey handling behaviors to cope with risks from large predators, we used a long-term data set from Serengeti National Park (SNP). Predation risk varies by age for cheetahs, as larger predators are the leading cause of cheetah cub death in SNP (Laurenson 1994), but adults are relatively safe from predation (Caro 1994). In SNP, cheetahs lose ~ 11% of their kills to lions and spotted hyenas (Hunter et al. 2007a). Some of the behaviors cheetahs use such as moving kills to longer grass and leaving immediately after eating can lower the probability of detection by larger carnivores, and therefore decrease the chances of kleptoparasitism and/or cub predation (Hunter et al. 2007a). However, a more detailed examination of how much time a cheetah spends on a carcass may reveal a tradeoff between avoiding kleptoparasitism and cub predation. Our study builds on Hunter et al. (2007a) work on the environmental and ecological factors that affect specific cheetah behaviors at the kill, to uncover whether cheetahs vary the speed at which they handle prey depending on whether cub predation or kleptoparasitism is the primary threat. In general, the more time spent handling prey, the greater chance of detection by larger carnivores. Therefore, we hypothesized that cheetahs without cubs (i.e., single females, single males, and male groups), whose main threat is kleptoparasitism, would adapt their behavior to minimize time spent handling prey. Losing a kill to larger predators has less effect on fitness than losing a cub; therefore, we expected that mothers with cubs would prioritize cub safety over quick nutritional gain and use prey handling behaviors at the safer end of the continuum. Mother cheetahs are vigilant at kills primarily to be able to protect cubs from incoming predators rather than to scan for prey (Caro 1987). Thus, we hypothesized that mothers would be more vigilant than cheetahs without cubs, primarily to minimize the risk of their cubs encountering predators.

The time cheetahs spend handling prey is made up of three major behaviors which are hunting, pausing before eating (during which cheetahs can recover breath, move the kill, and/or scan for predators), and eating. We predicted that to shorten handling time, cheetahs without cubs whose main threat is kleptoparasitism would spend less time pausing and/or eating than mothers with cubs. If cheetahs without cubs spent less time handling prey, we predicted that they would lose a lower percentage of their kills to lions and hyenas than mothers with cubs. To account for the contribution of time spent hunting to overall handling time, we also investigate whether time spent hunting differed between mothers and cheetahs without cubs.

Vigilance lengthens time spent handling prey but increases the chance that mothers will see approaching lions and spotted hyenas and be able to lead their cubs to safety. Therefore, we predicted that mothers would be more vigilant while eating, leading to longer eating times when compared to cheetahs without cubs. We also expected mothers to spend more time pausing to scan for predators before eating, which combined with longer eat times would lead to longer handling times.

We also test whether there was variation in prey handling strategy between mothers depending on cub age. Cubs younger than 4 months of age cannot run at full speed and are especially vulnerable to predation (Caro 1987). Therefore, we predicted that mothers with young cubs would prioritize behaviors that emphasize cub safety, such as being more vigilant while eating, which would lead to more time spent eating and handling prey than mothers with older cubs.

Material and methods

Study system

The Serengeti Cheetah Project (SCP) study site covers an area of 2200 km2 of open plains and woodland edge in the Serengeti National Park and Ngorongoro Conservation Area in Tanzania. Serengeti cheetahs are highly mobile and many follow the seasonal migration of Thomson’s gazelles (Eudorcas thomsonii), their main prey (Durant et al. 1988; Caro 1994). Detailed descriptions of the study site and ecosystem can be found in Sinclair and Arcese (1995).

Cheetah social system

Cheetahs have a unique social structure among cats, with multiple types of social groups (Caro 1994). Adult females are solitary unless they have dependent cubs. From birth until they are about 2 months old, cubs stay in the den and are not with their mother when she is hunting. Adult males can either be solitary or in lifelong coalitions with other males. We divided cheetahs into the following social groups: (i) mothers with following cubs up to 4 months of age, (ii) mothers with cubs older than 4 months, (iii) single females, (iv) single males, and (v) males in groups. Note that mothers with cubs in the den were classified as single females since cubs were not present while they handled prey.

Data collected

It was not possible to use a blinded methodology because our study involved focal animals in the field. We used observations of cheetah hunts by members of the Serengeti Cheetah Project (including TC, MKL, SMD, and AH) collected between 1980 and 2014. Serengeti cheetahs are mainly diurnal hunters and are usually habituated to vehicles, making it possible to directly observe and record their hunting behavior. We observed hunting behavior with binoculars to minimize disturbance and recorded the amount of time spent hunting, pausing, and eating in seconds (see Caro 1994). Handling time was defined as the time from when a cheetah started hunting (took two or more steps in an alert stalking gait towards prey), through the the chase and the kill, and ended when the cheetah was finished eating. Protocols for data collection on hunts used a standardized checksheet, and hence were standard across observers. Hunt time began at the start of the hunt and finished when the prey was immobilized (i.e., the cheetah has applied a stranglehold). Pause time started when the prey was dead (i.e., the cheetah dropped the stranglehold) to when the cheetah started to eat. Eat time was from the first bite taken to when the last bite was taken. If a cheetah stopped eating for an hour or more, we considered them to be finished eating. When we observed single females or single males, they were the focal animal. For mothers with cubs, the mother was always the focal animal, and the amount of time spent hunting, pausing, and eating represents her behavior. Males in groups usually hunt and eat together, and times recorded were for how long the group took to do a particular activity. Thus, handling time was from when the first male initiated a hunt to when the last male finished eating. Likewise, hunt time was from when a male initiated a hunt until he or another male applied the stranglehold to prey. Pause time was from when the stranglehold was dropped to when any of the males started to eat. Eat time was from when any male started eating until the last one had finished. We were not always able to collect data on all stages of handling time at every successful hunt; therefore, the number of observations for each stage varies (see supplementary Table S1).

For time spent vigilant, we used 3 years of data from MKL and 7 months of data from AH. MKL focused on females, while AH followed cheetahs opportunistically. Thus, the sample sizes for single females are larger than those for other social groups (see supplementary Table S1). Time spent vigilant (looking up from the carcass either while standing, sitting, or crouching) was recorded to the second for each individual except for mothers with cubs, when vigilance was only recorded for mothers. Vigilance was then calculated as a percentage of total time spent eating. For males in groups, we randomly chose data from one individual in the group to use in the analysis.

Statistics and modeling

Handling time was log transformed to achieve normality and used as the dependent variable in the models. Since there were multiple hunts by the same cheetah, we used linear mixed models with a coefficient representing the identity of cheetah as the random effect to avoid problems of pseudoreplication and to account for variation in hunting behavior among individual cheetahs. We included the following fixed effects in the models to account for the factors previously found to influence time spent handling prey in a variety of species including cheetahs (Croy and Hughes 1991; Bindoo and Aravindan 1992; Hilborn et al. 2012): social group, age of hunting cheetah (adolescent = 18 months to 2 years, young = 2–4 years, adult = 4 + years), hunger state, whether the kill was stolen, social and reproductive grouping, and the amount of meat available per cheetah. Short-term hunger state was determined by estimating belly size by eye on a 14 point scale (Caro 1994) and treated as a continuous variable. Whether the kill was stolen was a bivariate (yes/no) variable. We calculated the amount of meat available per cheetah by dividing the expected amount of meat from the carcass (estimated following Blumenschine and Caro (1986)) by the number of cheetahs present, except in the case of mothers with cubs. For mothers with cubs, we calculated the number of adult cheetah equivalents present at the kill. Following Caro (1994) and Laurenson (1995), we assumed that cubs’ food intake was proportional to their body height relative to that of their mother (for values used, see supplementary Table S2). Thus, if a mother and two half sized cubs ate at a kill, we considered the two cubs as one additional cheetah, and therefore, the food consumed was equivalent to two adult cheetahs. We log-transformed meat available per cheetah to achieve normality.

After modeling handling time as a whole, we further broke it into its consecutive behaviors to determine if the differences in handling time among social groups could be accounted for by differences in amount of time spent on the hunt, the pause before eating, or the time spent eating. The amounts of time spent hunting and pausing were not normally distributed; therefore, we used non-parametric Wilcoxon sum rank tests to check for significant differences in the median amount of time mothers with cubs spent in those activities compared to other social groups. We pooled mothers with cubs together and compared amount of time they spent in an activity to time spent by all other cheetahs grouped together. We then separately compared mothers with cubs to single females, single males, and male groups to test if time spent in the activity varied significantly among social groups.

To determine if mothers with cubs spent more time eating than other cheetahs, we used a mixed effects model with log transformed time spent eating as the dependent variable. We included a coefficient representing the identity of cheetah as the random effect, and our fixed effects were the factors identified as important in the handling time model, i.e., social group, meat available per cheetah, and whether or not the kill was stolen. In the model, we separated mothers into those with old versus young cubs. As with the handling time models, the variability explained by the fixed effects and the model as a whole was calculated using the method outlined in Nakagawa and Schielzeth (2013). We used a Chi-squared test to see if there were differences in rates of kleptoparasitism among social groups.

We log-transformed our data on proportion of time on a kill spent vigilant and used it as the dependent variable in mixed effects models. To test our a priori expectation that mothers with young cubs are more vigilant on a kill than those with old cubs, we first examined only kills made by mothers with cubs. In the model, we included a coefficient representing the identity of cheetah as the random effect, and our two fixed effects were a factor representing mothers with old versus young cubs, and prey size. Previous work shows that cheetahs are more vigilant on kills larger than 10 kg (Hunter et al. 2007a); therefore, we included a two-level factor for prey size (greater or less than 10 kg) according to Blumenschine and Caro (1986). We also combined all mothers with cubs together and used another mixed effects model with the same random and fixed effects, except that social group was a four-level factor with mothers with cubs compared to single males, single females, and males in groups.

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the appropriate author on reasonable request.

Results

Handling time

Total handling time for 351 successful hunts ranged from 6 to 530 min. The majority of handling time was spent eating prey, with the rest taken up by hunting and pausing before eating (Fig. 1). In our handling time model, significant factors were cheetah social group, meat available per cheetah, and whether or not the kill was stolen (Table 1). Age of cheetah and short-term hunger state did not significantly affect how much time cheetahs spend handling prey. Cheetahs without cubs had shorter handling times than mothers. Single males had the shortest handling time followed by male groups, than single females (Table 1). Mothers with young cubs spent significantly longer handling prey than mothers with old cubs (Table 1). The larger the kill, the longer the handling time, and if the kill was stolen, handling time was necessarily shortened (Table 1; Fig. 2). The fixed effects (i.e., amount of meat available per cheetah, social group, and whether the kill was stolen) explained 41.9% of the variation in the data, while the model as a whole (fixed effects plus the random effect of identity of cheetah) explained 56.8% of the variation, indicating that identity of individual cheetahs influenced model results.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mean time (untransformed) cheetahs spent on activities making up handling time, and handling time as a whole in Serengeti National Park in 1980–2014. Bars are standard deviation

Table 1 Effect size and significance of fixed effects in the handling time model. Log-transformed handling time is the dependent variable, ID of hunting cheetah is the random effect. Note: females with young cubs, male groups, single females, and single males are in comparison to females with old cubs (older than 4 months). Adult and young cheetahs are in comparison to adolescent cheetahs (18 months to 2 years old)
Fig. 2
figure 2

Model predictions from handling time model. Shows minutes spent handling (log -transformed) by social group and meat available per cheetah (log-transformed)

Hunting

Once we broke handling time into its constituent parts (i.e., hunting, pausing, eating), we found that the median amount of time mothers with cubs spent hunting was not significantly different than all other cheetahs combined. When we compared mothers to the different social groups separately, the only significant difference was that mothers had shorter hunts than male groups (Fig. 3a).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Median time spent hunting (a), pausing (b) between cheetah social groups. ES = Effect size and p value refer to the test of that social group against mothers with cubs. Cheetahs without cubs refer to pooling the data from single females, single males, and male groups. Values are from raw data

Pausing

Pause time ranged from − 8 min to over 2.5 h. The negative pause times were usually the result of one male in a group starting to eat before his brother had finished strangling the prey. However, some negative numbers came from single cheetahs who were ineffective at strangling and started to eat before prey were dead. Cheetahs without cubs (single females and males combined) paused for significantly less time (median = 3.9 min) than mothers with cubs (median = 11.4 min, p = 0.004, Fig. 3b). When comparing mothers with cubs to other social groups individually, mothers paused significantly longer than single males (median = 3.2 min, p = 0.012) and male groups (median = 0 min, p = < 0.001), but not single females (median = 8.1 min, p = 0.163).

Eating

Out of the 447 observations of time spent eating, 84% were of Thomson’s gazelle, ~ 10% were hares (Lepus spp.), with reedbuck (Redunca redunca), impala (Aepyceros melampus), and wildebeest (Connochaetes taurinus) making up the rest. Mothers with young cubs did not spent significantly longer eating than those with older cubs; however, single males, male groups, and single females spent less time eating than both mothers with young and old cubs (Table 2), though the difference between mothers with old cubs and male groups was not significant. The more meat that was available per cheetah, the longer they took to eat. The three fixed effects we included in our eat time model (social group, meat available per cheetah, and whether the kill was stolen) explained 37.7% of variability in the data, while the full model including identity of cheetah explained 48.4% of the variability. There were no significant differences in rates of kleptoparasitism among social groups (χ2 = 4.15, df = 4, p = 0.38).

Table 2 Effect size and significance of fixed effects in model of social grouping and meat available per cheetah on time spent eating (log-transformed). Note: females with young cubs, male groups, single males, single females are in comparison to mothers with old cubs (cubs > 4 months)

Vigilance while eating

Cheetahs were less vigilant on small kills than large ones though the difference was only marginally significant (Table 3). When prey size was accounted for, there was no significant difference in amount of time on a kill spent being vigilant between mothers with young versus old cubs (supplementary Table S3). When we grouped all mothers with cubs together, they spent significantly more time being vigilant than single males and single females, but not male groups (Table 3).

Table 3 Effect size and significance of fixed effects in model of social grouping and prey size on proportion of time on a kill spent vigilant. Note: male groups, single males, single females are in comparison to mothers with cubs (all ages). Small prey (< 10 kg flesh weight) is in comparison with prey > 10 kg flesh weight

Discussion

Our research reveals that aspects of cheetah prey handling behavior depend on risk from larger carnivores. Males and single females whose primary risk is kleptoparasitism have comparatively short overall handling times because they spend less time paused before eating and they eat relatively quickly. Mothers take a different approach since their primary threat is larger carnivores killing their cubs. Instead of speed, they use vigilance to minimize risk. They spend more time paused before eating and are more vigilant, increasing the amount of time they spend eating, which increases their overall handling time.

In order to reduce the chances of encountering large predators while hunting, mesopredators can avoid hunting when predators are nearby (Durant 1998; Cooper et al. 2007), or they can preferentially forage when the predators are less active (Harrington et al. 2009; Mukherjee et al. 2009). However, once prey are caught, there are other behaviors a mesopredator can use to lower the risks of predation and kleptoparasitism. When hunting large prey, maximizing nutritional gain requires spending substantial time handling the carcass, which increases the time spent in a risky situation. Moving the kill to a refuge is a strategy used by leopards (Panthera pardus) to lower rates of kleptoparasitism (Balme et al. 2017), while pumas (Puma concolor) cache large carcasses making their kills less likely to be detected by bears (Ursus americanus and arctos) (Murphy et al. 1998). Cheetahs cannot conceal their prey nor can they reliably defend their kills against larger predators and therefore they must employ different strategies, wile lions and hyenas are more likely to find and steal larger kills (Hunter et al. 2007b). Hayward et al. (2006) show that cheetahs do not preferentially select smaller prey to avoid kleptoparastism. Irrespective of size, to minimize the risk of their kill being stolen, they need to lower the chances of being detected by predators. Moving the kill to where it is better hidden by vegetation can extend the amount of time before it is discovered by hyenas (Hunter et al. 2007b), but regardless of habitat, decreasing handling time gives other predators less time to find the kill. When size of prey is taken into account, cheetahs without cubs decrease handling time by reducing time spent pausing after hunting, and reducing vigilance, which allows them to eat more quickly. Vigilance may enable a cheetah to see an approaching lion or spotted hyena, but it does not prevent the kill from being stolen. Although like Broekhuis et al. (2018), we found no significant differences in rates of kill loss by different cheetah social groups, out of 22 kills by single males in our dataset, none were lost to lions or hyenas. Habitat affects rates of kill loss (Hunter et al. 2007b); however, it is likely that spending the lowest amount of time eating and handling prey contributed to single males’ low rate of kleptoparasitism.

Since mothers with cubs on a kill face the risk of both predation and kleptoparasitism (Caro 1987), we might expect that they would also try to minimize time spent handling the carcass. A short handling time would reduce the chances of being discovered by lions and hyenas, lowering both risks. However, having cubs at the kill puts constraints on the ability of mothers to shorten their handling time. First, they have to make sure their cubs get enough to eat. Young cubs potentially slow down their mothers considerably since they have small mouths and are unfocused eaters, taking frequent breaks to rest and/or play (Caro 1994). Second, starting at ~ 4.5 months, the cubs practice chasing and killing live gazelle fawns brought to them by their mother (Caro 1995), which increases the time spent handling prey. However, it does not increase hunt time or pause time as it occurs after the prey is captured but before the prey is dead. A short handling time might minimize the time the cubs spend being vulnerable to predators, but it could compromise the cubs’ ability to eat to completion and to practice hunting. Contrary to our expectations, there were no significant differences in the amount of time mothers with young versus old cubs spent eating, pausing, or being vigilant. However, there were differences between mothers and cheetahs without cubs. In general, mothers were more vigilant, paused for longer before eating, and spent more time eating, which led to longer handling times than for cheetahs without cubs. The longer pauses shown by mothers may allow them to simultaneously take time for breath recovery while scanning for predators before starting to eat. Cheetahs without cubs do not pause as long, likely because predation is not a major threat and starting to eat quickly reduces the chance of kleptoparasitism. This suggests that mothers favor behaviors that slow down their handling time but keep them and their cubs safer, using vigilance to lessen the primary threat to the cubs while allowing them the time they need with prey.

Group size can affect the amount of time animals spend handling and eating prey through group vigilance (Lima 1995; Roberts 1996) and intragroup competition for food (Lamprecht 1978). Theoretically, group vigilance means each individual can be less vigilant while maintaining similar levels of safety, while intragroup competition for food favors those who eat quickly. Both of these factors should push males in groups to shorten their handling time. Yet, we found they eat more slowly and are more vigilant than single males, resulting in longer handling times. The explanation may lie in the multiple uses of vigilance, as Caro (1994) found that males use vigilance not as an anti-predator strategy but mainly to look out for potential mates. Thus, intragroup competition for mates may cause males to favor behaviors that result in a slower and more vigilant prey handling strategy. For male cheetahs, group living does not lead to reduced individual vigilance or less time spent eating as seen in many other species (Lima and Dill 1990).

The variety of risks cheetahs face from larger predators and the tradeoffs imposed by having cubs creates two broad prey handling strategies. A short handling time is favored by those primarily facing kleptoparasitism, while mothers slow down, taking time to be vigilant in order to lessen predation risk to their cubs. How cheetahs shorten their handling times varies by social group. For example, single males ate the fastest and were the least vigilant, while males in groups shorten their pauses instead of the time they spend eating. Individual identity also played a role in determining how long cheetahs spent eating and handling prey, suggesting that cheetahs display a continuum of prey handling and vigilance behaviors that individuals adapt depending on the risks and pressures they face at the kill. Therefore, we expect that these behaviors would vary in areas where the pressures on cheetahs are different. For example in Kgalagadi (Kalahari) Transfrontier Park (KTP) in South Africa and Botswana, lion densities are three times lower and spotted hyena densities are one hundred times lower than in SNP, and cheetah cub survival is eight times higher (Mills and Mills 2014). Thus, we might expect that lower risks to cubs from lions and hyenas at the kill in KTP will result in different prey handling behaviors by mothers compared to those in Serengeti, though this remains to be investigated. These flexible and individual strategies to minimize risk from apex predators likely contribute to successful coexistence of cheetahs with lions and spotted hyenas across a steep gradient of large carnivore densities.

How apex predators affect mesocarnivore prey handling behavior has implications beyond coexistence. Studies of mesopredator release have provided detail on how the reduction or extirpation of apex predator populations leads to mesopredators increasing predation pressure on prey species (Ritchie and Johnson 2009). Functional response models quantify how changes in predator foraging behaviors, such as handling time, affect the number of prey they kill (Beddington et al. 1976; Messier 1994; Murdoch et al. 2003). This provides a framework to help understand how mesopredator release can operate on a behavioral level. The role that apex predators play in shaping the functional response parameters of mesopredators indicates a mechanism for understanding the interactions among carnivores on multiple trophic levels together with their prey. Our work adds to the evidence that not only do other predators influence the functional response parameters of carnivores, but that the influence is not equal across individuals. Smith et al. (2015) found female pumas in California increased their kill rates to compensate for abandoning kills in areas with higher human housing density, while males did not. In this case, female pumas reacted to the increased pressure from a human ‘predator’ by having shorter handling times, leading to an increase in prey killed. Altering prey handling strategies along a continuum based on individual risk levels may aid mesocarnivores in coexisting with multiple apex predators, and be key to mesocarnivore survival, especially when spatial avoidance of predators is not possible (Durant 2000a, b).