We’re sorry, something doesn't seem to be working properly.

Please try refreshing the page. If that doesn't work, please contact support so we can address the problem.

Skip to main content
Log in

Selective phonotaxis by male wood frogs (Rana sylvatica) to the sound of a chorus

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Frogs and toads commonly form large choruses around suitable breeding habitat during the mating season. Although often regarded as a constraint on the acoustic behavior of signalers and receivers, the sounds of a chorus could also serve as an acoustic beacon that allows some frogs to locate the breeding aggregation. Attraction to chorus sounds might be particularly important for explosively breeding frogs. In these species, which often mate just one or a few days during the year, the timing and location of breeding aggregations can be unpredictable because their formation often depends on local climatic factors, such as rainfall or a change in temperature. I used laboratory playback experiments to test the hypothesis that male wood frogs (Rana sylvatica), an explosively breeding frog, exhibit positive phonotaxis toward the sound of a conspecific chorus. Males were released at the center of a rectangular arena with a speaker positioned in each corner facing toward the release point. In a single-stimulus experiment, more males approached a speaker broadcasting a conspecific chorus than the three silent speakers in the arena. In a two-stimulus experiment, more males approached a speaker broadcasting a conspecific chorus compared to the two silent speakers or a fourth speaker simultaneously broadcasting the spectrally overlapping sound of a heterospecific (R. septentrionalis) chorus. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that male wood frogs could use the sound of a chorus as a beacon to locate a short-lived breeding aggregation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amézquita A, Castellanos L, Hodl W (2005) Auditory matching of male Epipedobates femoralis (Anura: Dendrobatidae) under field conditions. Anim Behav 70:1377–1386

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bee MA, Gerhardt HC (2001a) Habituation as a mechanism of reduced aggression between neighboring territorial male bullfrogs (Rana catesbeiana). J Comp Psychol 115:68–82

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bee MA, Gerhardt HC (2001b) Neighbour–stranger discrimination by territorial male bullfrogs (Rana catesbeiana): I. Acoustic basis. Anim Behav 62:1129–1140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bee MA, Gerhardt HC (2001c) Neighbour–stranger discrimination by territorial male bullfrogs (Rana catesbeiana): II. Perceptual basis. Anim Behav 62:1141–1150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bee MA, Gerhardt HC (2002) Individual voice recognition in a territorial frog (Rana catesbeiana). Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 269:1443–1448

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bee MA, Perrill SA, Owen PC (1999) Size assessment in simulated territorial encounters between male green frogs (Rana clamitans). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 45:177–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bee MA, Perrill SA, Owen PC (2000) Male green frogs lower the pitch of acoustic signals in defense of territories: a possible dishonest signal of size? Behav Ecol 11:169–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berven KA (1981) Mate choice in the wood frog, Rana sylvatica. Evolution 35:707–722

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boatright-Horowitz SS, Cheney CA, Simmons AM (1999) Atmospheric and underwater propagation of bullfrog vocalizations. Bioacoustics 9:257–280

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogert CM (1960) The influence of sounds on the behavior of amphibians and reptiles. In: Lanyon WE, Tavolga WN (eds) Animal sounds and communication. American Institute of Biological Sciences, Washington, DC, pp 137–320

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourne GR, Collins AC, Holder AM, McCarthy CL (2001) Vocal communication and reproductive behavior of the frog Colostethus beebei in Guyana. J Herpetol 35:272–281

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brenowitz EA (1989) Neighbor call amplitude influences aggressive behavior and intermale spacing in choruses of the Pacific treefrog (Hyla regilla). Ethology 83:69–79

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooke PN, Alford RA, Schwarzkopf L (2000) Environmental and social factors influence chorusing behaviour in a tropical frog: examining various temporal and spatial scales. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 49:79–87

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bush SL, Dyson ML, Halliday TR (1996) Selective phonotaxis by males in the Majorcan midwife toad. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 263:913–917

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell JP (1997) Pair bonding in spotted poison frogs. Nature 385:211

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Capranica RR, Moffat JM (1983) Neurobehavioral correlates of sound communication in anurans. In: Ewert JP, Capranica RR, Ingle DJ (eds) Advances in vertebrate neuroethology. Plenum, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies NB, Halliday TR (1978) Deep croaks and fighting assessment in toads Bufo bufo. Nature 274:683–685

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis MS (1987) Acoustically mediated neighbor recognition in the North American bullfrog, Rana catesbeiana. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 21:185–190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diego-Rasilla FJ, Luengo RM (2004) Heterospecific call recognition and phonotaxis in the orientation behavior of the marbled newt, Triturus marmoratus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:556–560

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott L (2004) The calls of frogs and toads. Stackpole, Mechanicsburg, PA

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedl TWP, Klump GM (2002) The vocal behaviour of male European treefrogs (Hyla arborea): implications for inter- and intrasexual selection. Behaviour 139:113–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt HC (1975) Sound pressure levels and radiation patterns of vocalizations of some North American frogs and toads. J Comp Physiol 102:1–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt HC (1995) Phonotaxis in female frogs and toads: execution and design of experiments. In: Klump GM, Dooling RJ, Fay RR, Stebbins WC (eds) Methods in comparative psychoacoustics. Birkhäuser Verlag, Basel, pp 209–220

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt HC (2001) Acoustic communication in two groups of closely related treefrogs. In: Advances in the study of behavior, vol 30, pp 99–167

  • Gerhardt HC, Bee MA (2006) Recognition and localization of acoustic signals. In: Narins PM, Feng AS, Fay RR, Popper AN (eds) Hearing and sound communication in amphibians, vol 28. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 113–146

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt HC, Huber F (2002) Acoustic communication in insects and anurans: common problems and diverse solutions. Chicago University Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt HC, Klump GM (1988a) Masking of acoustic signals by the chorus background noise in the green treefrog: a limitation on mate choice. Anim Behav 36:1247–1249

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt HC, Klump GM (1988b) Phonotactic responses and selectivity of barking treefrogs (Hyla gratiosa) to chorus sounds. J Comp Physiol A 163:795–802

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grafe TU (1997) Costs and benefits of mate choice in the lek-breeding reed frog, Hyperolius marmoratus. Anim Behav 53:1103–1117

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauselberger KF, Alford RA (2005) Effects of season and weather on calling in the Australian microhylid frogs Austrochaperina robusta and Cophixalus ornatus. Herpetologica 61:349–363

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henzi SP, Dyson ML, Piper SE, Passmore NE, Bishop P (1995) Chorus attendance by male and female painted reed frogs (Hyperolius marmoratus): Environmental factors and selection pressures. Funct Ecol 9:485–491

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard RD (1978) Evolution of mating strategies in bullfrogs, Rana catesbeiana. Evolution 32:850–871

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard RD (1980) Mating behavior and mating success in wood frogs, Rana sylvatica. Anim Behav 28:705–716

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard RD, Kluge AG (1985) Proximate mechanisms of sexual selection in wood frogs. Evolution 39:260–277

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard RD, Palmer JG (1995) Female choice in Bufo americanus: Effects of dominant frequency and call order. Copeia 212–217

  • Howard RD, Young JR (1998) Individual variation in male vocal traits and female mating preferences in Bufo americanus. Animal Behaviour 55:1165–1179

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kroodsma DE (1989) Suggested experimental designs for song playbacks. Anim Behav 37:600–609

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus-Hills JJ, Littlejohn MJ (1971) Mating-call sound intensities of anuran amphibians. J Acoust Soc Am 49:1327–1329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall VT, Humfeld SC, Bee MA (2003) Plasticity of aggressive signalling and its evolution in male spring peepers, Pseudacris crucifer. Anim Behav 65:1223–1234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martof BS, Humphries RL (1959) Geographic variation in the woodfrog, Rana sylvatica. Am Midl Nat 61:350–389

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy CG (2003) The cause of correlations between nightly numbers of male and female barking treefrogs (Hyla gratiosa) attending choruses. Behav Ecol 14:274–281

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy CG, Gerhardt HC (2002) Mate sampling by female barking treefrogs (Hyla gratiosa). Behav Ecol 13:472–480

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narins PM (1982) Effects of masking noise on evoked calling in the Puerto Rican coqui (Anura, Leptodactylidae). J Comp Physiol 147:439–446

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narins PM, Hodl W, Grabul DS (2003) Bimodal signal requisite for agonistic behavior in a dart-poison frog, Epipedobates femoralis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:577–580

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield B, Moriarty JJ (1994) Amphibians and reptiles native to Minnesota. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis

    Google Scholar 

  • Oldham RS (1966) Spring movements in the American toad, Bufo americanus. Can J Zool 44:63–100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oldham RS (1967) Orienting mechanisms of the green frog, Rana clamitans. Ecology 48:477–491

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oseen KL, Wassersug RJ (2002) Environmental factors influencing calling in sympatric anurans. Oecologia 133:616–625

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penna M, Solis R (1998) Frog call intensities and sound propagation in the South American temperate forest region. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42:371–381

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petranka JW, Thomas DAG (1995) Explosive breeding reduces egg and tadpole cannibalism in the wood frog, Rana sylvatica. Anim Behav 50:731–739

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfenning KS, Rapa K, McNatt R (2000) Evolution of male mating behavior: male spadefoot toads preferentially associate with conspecific males. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48:69–74

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Regosin JV, Windmiller BS, Reed JM (2003) Terrestrial habitat use and winter densities of the wood frog (Rana sylvatica). J Herpetol 37:390–394

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson JGM (1986) Male territoriality, fighting and assessment of fighting ability in the Australian frog Uperoleia rugosa. Anim Behav 34:763–772

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ (1985) The Túngara frog: a study in sexual selection and communication. Chicago University Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ (ed) (2001) Anuran communication. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, D.C.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ, Rand AS (1993) Species recognition and sexual selection as a unitary problem in animal communication. Evolution 47:647–657

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz JJ (1994) Male advertisement and female choice in frogs: recent findings and new approaches to the study of communication in a dynamic acoustic environment. Am Zool 34:616–624

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz JJ, Buchanan BW, Gerhardt HC (2001) Female mate choice in the gray treefrog (Hyla versicolor) in three experimental environments. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 49:443–455

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens CE, Paszkowski CA (2004) Using chorus-size ranks from call surveys to estimate reproductive activity of the wood frog (Rana sylvatica). J Herpetol 38:404–410

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaira M (2005) Annual variation of breeding patterns of the toad, Melanophryniscus rubriventris (Vellard, 1947). Amphib–Reptil 26:193–199

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner WE (1989) Fighting, assessment, and frequency alteration in Blanchard cricket frog. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 25:429–436

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waldman B (1982) Adaptive significance of communal oviposition in wood frogs (Rana sylvatica). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 10

  • Weir LA, Mossman MJ (2005) North American Amphibian Monitoring Prgram (NAAMP). In: Lannoo MJ (ed) Amphibian declines: conservation status of United States species. University of California Press, Berkeley, pp 307–313

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD (1977a) The social behaviour of anuran amphibians. Anim Behav 25:666–693

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD (1977b) Territoriality and male mating success in green frog (Rana clamitans). Ecology 58:750–762

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD (1978) Territoriality in the green frog (Rana clamitans): vocalizations and agonistic behavior. Anim Behav 26:1051

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells KD, Schwartz JJ (2006) The behavioral ecology of anuran communication. In: Narins PM, Feng AS, Fay RR, Popper AN (eds) Hearing and sound communication in amphibians, vol 28. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 44–86

  • Wollerman L (1999) Acoustic interference limits call detection in a neotropical frog Hyla ebraccata. Anim Behav 57:529–536

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wollerman L, Wiley RH (2002) Background noise from a natural chorus alters female discrimination of male calls in a neotropical frog. Anim Behav 63:15–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woolbright LL, Greene EJ, Rapp GC (1990) Density-dependent mate searching strategies of male woodfrogs. Anim Behav 40:135–142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zakon HH, Wilczynski W (1988) The physiology of the anuran eighth nerve. In: Fritzsch B, Wolkowiak W, Ryan MJ, Wilczynski W, Hetherington T (eds) The evolution of the amphibian auditory system. Wiley, New York, pp 125–155

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

I thank John Moriarty and the Ramsey County Parks Department for access to the Tamarack Nature Center. Eli Swanson and Sandra Tekmen provided helpful assistance collecting and testing frogs. Alejandro Velez, Vince Marshall, and two anonymous referees provided helpful feedback on previous versions of the manuscript. This work was approved by the University of Minnesota’s IACUC (#0510A76966) on December 15, 2005, conducted under Special Permit 13313 from the Minnesota Department of Natural Resources, and complied with all laws of the USA.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mark A. Bee.

Additional information

Communicated by J. Christensen-Dalsgaard

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bee, M.A. Selective phonotaxis by male wood frogs (Rana sylvatica) to the sound of a chorus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61, 955–966 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0324-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0324-8

Keywords

Navigation