Skip to main content
Log in

Unintentional drifts during quiet stance and voluntary body sway

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We explored unintentional drifts in voluntary whole-body sway tasks following the removal of visual feedback. The main hypothesis was that the unintentional drifts were produced by drifts of referent coordinates for salient performance variables. Young healthy subjects stood quietly on a force platform and also performed voluntary body sway at 0.5 Hz both in the anterio-posterior and medio-lateral directions. Visual feedback on the center of pressure (COP) coordinate was provided and then turned off. During quiet stance trials, the subjects matched the initial COP coordinate with a target shifted by 3 cm anterior, posterior, left, or right from the coordinate during natural standing and activated the right tibialis anterior to 30% of its maximal voluntary contraction. During cyclical voluntary sway task, the nominal sway amplitude was always 4 cm while the midpoint was at one of the four mentioned locations. Removing visual feedback caused COP drifts during quiet stance trials that were consistent across trials performed by a subject but could be in opposite directions across subjects; there was a consistent drop in the activation level of tibialis anterior. During voluntary body sway, removing visual feedback caused a consistent increase in the voluntary sway amplitude and a drift of the midpoint that was consistent within but not across subjects. Motor equivalent and non-motor equivalent inter-cycle motion components were quantified within the space of muscle groups (muscle modes) under visual feedback and at the end of the period without visual feedback. Throughout the trial, there were large motor equivalent motion components, and they increased over the period without visual feedback. The results corroborate the idea that referent coordinate drifts at different levels of the control hierarchy can lead to unintentional drifts in performance. It suggests that directions of COP drifts are defined by two main factors, drift of the body referent coordinate toward the actual coordinate (that can lead to fall) and an opposite drift to ensure body motion to a safer location. Analysis of motor equivalence suggests that postural stability is not compromised during unintentional drifts in performance in contrast to earlier studies of multi-finger tasks. This may be due to the vital importance of postural stability for everyday actions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alexander MR (2002) Energetics and optimization of human walking and running: the 2000 Raymond Pearl memorial lecture. Am J Hum Biol 14:641–648

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambike S, Paclet F, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2014) Factors affecting grip force: anatomy, mechanics, and referent configurations. Exp Brain Res 232:1219–1231

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ambike S, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2015) Processes underlying unintentional finger-force changes in the absence of visual feedback. Exp Brain Res 233:711–721

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ambike S, Mattos D, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2016) The nature of constant and cyclic force production: unintentional force-drift characteristics. Exp Brain Res 234:197–208

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ambike S, Mattos D, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2017) Unsteady steady states: central causes of unintentional force drift. Exp Brain Res 234:3597–3611

    Google Scholar 

  • Błaszczyk JW (2016) The use of force-plate posturography in the assessment of postural instability. Gait Posture 44:1–6

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blaszczyk JW, Prince F, Raiche M, Hébert R (2000) Effect of ageing and vision on limb load asymmetry during quiet stance. J Biomech 33:1243–1248

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corcos DM, Gottlieb GL, Latash ML, Almeida GL, Agarwal GC (1992) Electromechanical delay: an experimental artifact. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 2:59–68

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danna-Dos-Santos A, Slomka K, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2007) Muscle modes and synergies during voluntary body sway. Exp Brain Res 179:533–550

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danna-Dos-Santos A, Degani AM, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2008) Is voluntary control of natural postural sway possible? J Mot Behav 40:179–185

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Duarte M, Zatsiorsky VM (1999) Patterns of center of presure migration during prolonged unconstrained standing. Mot Control 3:12–27

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Duarte M, Zatsiorsky VM (2000) On the fractal properties of natural human standing. Neurosci Lett 283:173–176

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG (1966) Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of movement or maintenance of a steady posture. II. Controllable parameters of the muscle. Biophysics 11:565–578

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG (1986) Once more on the equilibrium-point hypothesis (λ-model) for motor control. J Mot Behav 18:17–54

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman AG (2015) Referent control of action and perception: Challenging conventional theories in behavioral science. Springer, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraizer EV, Mitra S (2008) Methodological and interpretive issues in posture-cognition dual-tasking in upright stance. Gait Posture 27:271–279

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haddad JM, Rietdyk S, Ryu JH, Seaman JM, Silver TA, Kalish JA, Hughes CML (2011) Postural asymmetries in response to holding evenly and unevenly distributed loads during self-selected stance. J Mot Behav 43:345–355

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hair JF, Anderson RE, Tatham RL, Black WC (1995) Factor analysis. In: Borkowski D (ed) Multivariate data analysis. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, pp 364–404

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatsopoulos NG, Warren WH Jr (1996) Resonance tuning in rhythmic arm movements. J Mot Behav 28:3–14

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heijnen MJ, Muir BC, Rietdyk S (2012) Factors leading to obstacle contact during adaptive locomotion. Exp Brain Res 223:219–231

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heijnen MJ, Romine NL, Stumpf DM, Rietdyk S (2014) Memory-guided obstacle crossing: more failures were observed for the trail limb versus lead limb. Exp Brain Res 232:2131–2142

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horak FB, Nutt JG, Nashner LM (1992) Postural inflexibility in parkinsonian subjects. J Neurol Sci 111:46–58

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jo HJ, Park J, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML (2015) Prehension synergies and hand function in early-stage Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res 233:425–440

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jo HJ, Ambike S, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML (2016) Finger force changes in the absence of visual feedback in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Clin Neurophysiol 127:684–692

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kawato M (1999) Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 9:718–727

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kay BA, Kelso JA, Saltzman EL, Schoner G (1987) Space-time behavior of single and bimanual rhythmical movements: data and limit cycle model. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 13:178–192

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kendall FP, McCreary EK, Provance PG, Rodgers MM, Romani WA (2005) Muscles: testing and function with posture and pain, 5th edn. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiemel T, Oie KS, Jeka JJ (2002) Multisensory fusion and the stochastic structure of postural sway. Biol Cybern 87:262–277

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klous M, Danna-dos-Santos A, Latash ML (2010) Multi-muscle synergies in a dual postural task: evidence for the principle of superposition. Exp Brain Res 202:457–471

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Klous M, Mikulic P, Latash ML (2011) Two aspects of feed-forward postural control: anticipatory postural adjustments and anticipatory synergy adjustments. J Neurophysiol 105:2275–2288

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Krakauer JW, Mazzoni P (2011) Human sensorimotor learning: adaptation, skill, and beyond. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21:636–644

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krishnamoorthy V, Goodman SR, Latash ML, Zatsiorsky VM (2003a) Muscle synergies during shifts of the center of pressure by standing persons: identification of muscle modes. Biol Cybern 89:152–161

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krishnamoorthy V, Latash ML, Scholz JP, Zatsiorsky VM (2003b) Muscle synergies during shifts of the center of pressure by standing persons. Exp Brain Res 152:281–292

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML (2010) Motor synergies and the equilibrium-point hypothesis. Mot Control 14:294–322

    Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML (2016) Towards physics of neural processes and behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 69:136–146

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML (2017) Biological movement and laws of physics. Motor Control (in press)

  • Latash ML, Zatsiorsky VM (1993) Joint stiffness: myth or reality? Hum Move Sci 12:653–692

    Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML, Zatsiorsky VM (2016) Biomechanics and motor control: defining central concepts. Academic, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML, Scholz JP, Schoner G (2007) Toward a new theory of motor synergies. Mot Control 11:276–308

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattos D, Latash ML, Park E, Kuhl J, Scholz JP (2011) Unpredictable elbow joint perturbation during reaching results in multijoint motor equivalence. J Neurophysiol 106:1424–1436

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mattos D, Kuhl J, Scholz JP, Latash ML (2013) Motor equivalence (ME) during reaching: is ME observable at the muscle level? Mot Control 17:145–175

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattos D, Schöner G, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2015) Task-specific stability of abundant systems: structure of variance and motor equivalence. Neurosci 310:600–615

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mochizuki L, Duarte M, Amadio AC, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2006) Changes in postural sway and its fractions in conditions of postural instability. J Appl Biomech 22:51–66

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson W (1983) Physical principles for economies of skilled movements. Biol Cybern 46:135–147

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Park J, Wu Y-H, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML (2012) Changes in multi-finger interaction and coordination in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurophysiol 108:915–924

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Parsa B, O’Shea DJ, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2016) On the nature of unintentional action: a study of force/moment drifts during multi-finger tasks. J Neurophysiol 116:698–708

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Parsa B, Terekhov AV, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2017a) Optimality and stability of intentional and unintentional actions: I. Origins of drifts in performance. Exp Brain Res 235:481–496

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parsa B, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2017b) Optimality and stability of intentional and unintentional actions: II. Motor equivalence and structure of variance. Exp Brain Res 235:457–470

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piscitelli D, Falaki A, Solnik S, Latash ML (2017) Anticipatory postural adjustments and anticipatory synergy adjustments: preparing to a postural perturbation with predictable and unpredictable direction. Exp Brain Res 235:713–730

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poon C, Chin-Cottongim LG, Coombes SA, Corcos DM, Vaillancourt DE (2012) Spatiotemporal dynamics of brain activity during the transition from visually guided to memory-guided force control. J Neurophysiol 108:1335–1348

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Prilutsky BI, Zatsiorsky VM (2002) Optimization-based models of muscle coordination. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 30:32–38

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Reschechtko S, Hasanbarani F, Akulin VM, Latash ML (2017) Unintentional force changes in cyclical tasks performed by an abundant system: Empirical observations and a dynamical model. Neurosci 350:94–109

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Riccio GE (1993) Information in movement variability about the qualitative dynamics of posture and orientation. In: Newell KM, Corcos DM (eds) Variability and motor control. Human Kinetics Publ, Champaign, pp 317–358

    Google Scholar 

  • Riley MA, Wong S, Mitra S, Turvey MT (1997) Common effects of touch and vision on postural parameters. Exp Brain Res 117:165–170

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robert T, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2008) Multi-muscle synergies in an unusual postural task: quick shear force production. Exp Brain Res 187:237–253

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ruffieux J, Keller M, Lauber B, Taube W (2015) Changes in standing and walking performance under dual-task conditions across the lifespan. Sports Med 45:1739–1758

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sainburg RL (2002) Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness. Exp Brain Res 142:241–258

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sainburg RL (2005) Handedness: differential specializations for control of trajectory and position. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 33:206–213

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz JP, Schoner G (1999) The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task. Exp Brain Res 126:289–306

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz JP, Schöner G, Hsu WL, Jeka JJ, Horak F, Martin V (2007) Motor equivalent control of the center of mass in response to support surface perturbations. Exp Brain Res 180:163–179

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schoner G (1995) Recent developments and problems in human movement science and their conceptual implications. Ecol Psychol 8:291–314

    Google Scholar 

  • Shadmehr R, Wise SP (2005) The computational neurobiology of reaching and pointing. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapkova EYu, Shapkova AL, Goodman SR, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2008) Do synergies decrease force variability? A study of single-finger and multi-finger force production. Exp Brain Res 188:411–425

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Slifkin AB, Vaillancourt DE, Newell KM (2000) Intermittency in the control of continuous force production. J Neurophysiol 84:1708–1718

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Solnik S, Qiao M, Latash ML (2017) Effects of visual feedback and memory on unintentional drifts in performance during finger pressing tasks. Exp Brain Res 235:1149–1162

  • Vaillancourt DE, Russell DM (2002) Temporal capacity of short-term visuomotor memory in continuous force production. Exp Brain Res 145:275–285

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vaillancourt DE, Slifkin AB, Newell KM (2001) Visual control of isometric force in Parkinson’s disease. Neurophysiologia 39:1410–1418

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vaillancourt DE, Thulborn KR, Corcos DM (2003) Neural basis for the processes that underlie visually guided and internally guided force control in humans. J Neurophysiol 90:3330–3340

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang Y, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2005) Muscle synergies involved in shifting center of pressure during making a first step. Exp Brain Res 167:196–210

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang Y, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2006) Muscle synergies in preparation to a step made under self-paced and reaction-time instructions. Clin Neurophysiol 117:41–56

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winter DA (2009) Biomechanics and motor control of human movement, 4th edn. NY, Wiley

    Google Scholar 

  • Winter DA, Prince F, Frank JS, Powell C, Zabjek KF (1996) Unified theory regarding A/P and M/L balance in quiet stance. J Neurophysiol 75:2334–2343

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert DM, Miall RC, Kawato M (1998) Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn Sci 2:338–347

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yavari F, Towhidkhah F, Ahmadi-Pajouh MA (2013) Are fast/slow process in motor adaptation and forward/inverse internal model two sides of the same coin? Med Hypotheses 81:592–600

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zatsiorsky VM, Duarte M (2000) Rambling and trembling in quiet standing. Mot Control 4:185–200

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou T, Solnik S, Wu Y-H, Latash ML (2014) Unintentional movements produced by back-coupling between the actual and referent body configurations: violations of equifinality in multi-joint positional tasks. Exp Brain Res 232:3847–3859

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou T, Zhang L, Latash ML (2015) Characteristics of unintentional movements by a multi-joint effector. J Mot Behav 47:352–361

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The study was in part supported by an NIH Grant R01 NS035032.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mark L. Latash.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rasouli, O., Solnik, S., Furmanek, M.P. et al. Unintentional drifts during quiet stance and voluntary body sway. Exp Brain Res 235, 2301–2316 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-4972-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-4972-x

Keywords

Navigation