Skip to main content
Log in

Position sense at the human elbow joint measured by arm matching or pointing

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Position sense at the human elbow joint has traditionally been measured in blindfolded subjects using a forearm matching task. Here we compare position errors in a matching task with errors generated when the subject uses a pointer to indicate the position of a hidden arm. Evidence from muscle vibration during forearm matching supports a role for muscle spindles in position sense. We have recently shown using vibration, as well as muscle conditioning, which takes advantage of muscle’s thixotropic property, that position errors generated in a forearm pointing task were not consistent with a role by muscle spindles. In the present study we have used a form of muscle conditioning, where elbow muscles are co-contracted at the test angle, to further explore differences in position sense measured by matching and pointing. For fourteen subjects, in a matching task where the reference arm had elbow flexor and extensor muscles contracted at the test angle and the indicator arm had its flexors conditioned at 90°, matching errors lay in the direction of flexion by 6.2°. After the same conditioning of the reference arm and extension conditioning of the indicator at 0°, matching errors lay in the direction of extension (5.7°). These errors were consistent with predictions based on a role by muscle spindles in determining forearm matching outcomes. In the pointing task subjects moved a pointer to align it with the perceived position of the hidden arm. After conditioning of the reference arm as before, pointing errors all lay in a more extended direction than the actual position of the arm by 2.9°–7.3°, a distribution not consistent with a role by muscle spindles. We propose that in pointing muscle spindles do not play the major role in signalling limb position that they do in matching, but that other sources of sensory input should be given consideration, including afferents from skin and joint.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen TJ, Proske U (2006) Effect of muscle fatigue on the sense of limb position and movement. Exp Brain Res 170:30–38

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allen TJ, Ansems GE, Proske U (2007) Effects of muscle conditioning on position sense at the human forearm during loading or fatigue of elbow flexors and the role of the sense of effort. J Physiol 580(2):423–434

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bock O, Pipereit K, Mierau A (2007) A method to reversibly degrade proprioceptive feedback in research on human motor control. J Neurosci Methods 160(2):246–250

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boisgontier MP, Nougier V (2013) Proprioception: bilateral inputs first. Neurosci Lett 534:96–100

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown MC, Engberg I, Matthews PB (1967) Fusimotor stimulation and the dynamic sensitivity of the secondary ending of the muscle spindle. J Physiol 189(3):545–550

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brun C, Guerraz M (2015) Anchoring the “floating arm”: use of proprioceptive and mirror visual feedback from one arm to control involuntary displacement of the other arm. Neuroscience 310:268–278

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark FJ, Burgess RC, Chapin JW, Lipscomb WT (1985) Role of intramuscular receptors in the awareness of limb position. J Neurophysiol 54(6):1529–1540

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collins DF, Prochazka A (1996) Movement illusions evoked by ensemble cutaneous input from the dorsum of the human hand. J Physiol 496(Pt 3):857–871

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Collins DF, Refshauge KM, Todd G, Gandevia SC (2005) Cutaneous receptors contribute to kinesthesia at the index finger, elbow, and knee. J Neurophysiol 94(3):1699–1706

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrell WR, Smith A (1988) Position sense at the proximal interphalangeal joint of the human index finger. J Physiol 399:49–61

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fuentes CT, Bastian AJ (2010) Where is your arm? Variations in proprioception across space and tasks. J Neurophysiol 103(1):164–171

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilhodes JC, Roll JP, Tardy-Gervet MF (1986) Perceptual and motor effects of agonist–antagonist muscle vibration in man. Exp Brain Res 61(2):395–402

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Givoni NJ, Pham T, Allen TJ, Proske U (2007) The effect of quadriceps muscle fatigue on position matching at the knee. J Physiol 584(1):111–119

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Goble DJ, Lewis CA, Brown SH (2006) Upper limb asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback. Exp Brain Res 168(1–2):307–311

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goble DJ, Coxon JP, Wenderoth N, Van Impe A, Swinnen SP (2009) Proprioceptive sensibility in the elderly: degeneration, functional consequences and plastic-adaptive processes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33(3):271–278

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin GM, McCloskey DI, Matthews PB (1972) The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. Brain 95(4):705–748

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gooey K, Bradfield O, Talbot J, Morgan DL, Proske U (2000) Effects of body orientation, load and vibration on sensing movement and position at the human elbow joint. Exp Brain Res 133:340–348

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory JE, Morgan DL, Proske U (1988) Aftereffects in the responses of cat muscle spindles and errors of limb position sense in man. J Neurophysiol 59(4):1220–1230

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hakuta N, Izumizaki M, Kigawa K, Murai N, Atsumi T, Homma I (2014) Proprioceptive illusions created by vibration of one arm are altered by vibrating the other arm. Exp Brain Res 232(7):2197–2206

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Izumizaki M, Tsuge M, Akai L, Proske U, Homma I (2010) The illusion of changed position and movement from vibrating one arm is altered by vision or movement of the other arm. J Physiol 588(15):2789–2800

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kammers MPM, van der Ham IJM, Dijkerman HC (2006) Dissociating body representations in healthy individuals: differential effects of a kinaesthetic illusion on perception and action. Neuropsychologia 44:2430–2436

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kigawa K, Izumizaki M, Tsukada S, Hakuta N (2015) Proprioceptive interaction between the two arms in a single-arm pointing task. PLoS ONE 10(8):e0137031

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Longo MR (2014) The effects of immediate vision on implicit hand maps. Exp Brain Res 232(4):1241–1247

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Longo MR, Azanon E, Haggard P (2010) More than skin deep: body representation beyond primary somatosensory cortex. Neuropsychologia 48(3):655–668

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews PBC (1988) Proprioceptors and their contribution to somatosensory mapping: complex messages require complex processing. Can J Physiol Pharmcol 66:430–438

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey DI (1973) Differences between the sense of movement and position shown by the effects of loading and vibration of muscles in man. Brain Res 61:119–131

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller LE, Longo MR, Saygin AP (2016) Mental body representations retain homuncular shape distortions: evidence from Weber’s illusion. Conscious Cogn 40:17–25

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proske U, Gandevia SC (2012) The proprioceptive senses: their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force. Physiol Rev 92(4):1651–1697

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proske U, Morgan DL, Gregory JE (1992) Muscle history dependence of responses to stretch of primary and secondary endings of cat soleus muscle spindles. J Physiol 445:81–95

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Proske U, Morgan DL, Gregory JE (1993) Thixotropy in skeletal muscle and in muscle spindles: a review. Prog Neurobiol 41(6):705–721

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proske U, Tsay A, Allen T (2014) Muscle thixotropy as a tool in the study of proprioception. Exp Brain Res 232(11):3397–3412

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roll JP, Vedel JP, Ribot E (1989) Alteration of proprioceptive messages induced by tendon vibration in man: a microneurographic study. Exp Brain Res 76(1):213–222

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsay A, Savage G, Allen TJ, Proske U (2014) Limb position sense, proprioceptive drift and muscle thixotropy at the human elbow joint. J Physiol 592(Pt 12):2679–2694

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tsay A, Allen TJ, Proske U (2015) Position sense at the human forearm after conditioning elbow muscles with isometric contractions. Exp Brain Res 233(9):2635–2643

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsay A, Giummarra M, Allen TJ, Proske U (2016) The sensory origins of human position sense. J Physiol 594(4):1037–1049

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsuge M, Izumizaki M, Kigawa K, Atsumi T, Homma I (2012) Interaction between vibration-evoked proprioceptive illusions and mirror-evoked visual illusions in an arm-matching task. Exp Brain Res 223(4):541–551

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Velay JL, Roll J, Paillard J (1989) Elbow position sense in man: contrasting results in matching and pointing. Hum Mov Sci 8:177–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh LD, Proske U, Allen TJ, Gandevia SC (2013) The contribution of motor commands to position sense differs between elbow and wrist. J Physiol 591(Pt 23):6103–6114

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • White O, Proske U (2009) Illusions of forearm displacement during vibration of elbow muscles in humans. Exp Brain Res 192(1):113–120

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Uwe Proske.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tsay, A., Allen, T.J. & Proske, U. Position sense at the human elbow joint measured by arm matching or pointing. Exp Brain Res 234, 2787–2798 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4680-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4680-y

Keywords

Navigation