Skip to main content
Log in

Adaptation to rotated visual feedback depends on the number and spread of target directions

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It has been proposed in the past that adaptation to rotated visual feedback is based on directionally tuned modules. Here, we investigate whether adaptation depends on the number of modules that are concurrently activated. To disambiguate the number of modules and their spatial overlap, we decided to vary the number of target directions and their spacing independently. In light of recent work on the existence of fast and slow adaptive processes, we analyzed the role of target number and spacing separately for the first eight movements under rotated visual feedback and for later movements. We found that during the first eight movements, adaptation progressed three times faster when targets were spaced across a 42° rather than across a 360° range, irrespective of their number. During the subsequent movements, adaptation progressed 1.66 times faster with two than with eight targets, irrespective of their spacing. This differential dependence of early and late adaptation on target metrics confirms the existence of adaptive processes with different time scales and suggests that those processes differ not only by their clocking speed, but also by their functional properties. Specifically, the speed of fast processes seems to be constrained by the directional tuning width of adaptive modules, and the speed of slow processes by the number of presentations per target direction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Randomization was performed with the in C++ statement “rand” after seeding with “srand”.

  2. Movement onset was defined by a velocity threshold of 30 mm/s.

  3. We decided for eight movements to represent fast adaptive processes since all target directions were presented equally often within eight movements (see "Procedures"). This is slightly less than the ten or twelve movements selected in earlier studies on adaptation to mirror reversals (Stratton et al. 2007) and force fields (Smith et al. 2006), respectively. Since the first eight adaptation movements in the present study took about 30 s, our choice is comparable to the value determined for fast adaptation to visual rotations in psychophysical experiments (Inoue et al. 2000). We are therefore confident that our analysis didn’t exceed the temporal limits of fast processes.

References

  • Abeele S, Bock O (2001) Mechanisms for sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input. Exp Brain Res 139:248–253

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Andersen RA, Bracewell RM, Barash S, Gnadt JW, Fogassi L (1990) Eye position effects on visual, memory, and saccade-related activity in areas LIP and 7a of macaque. J Neurosci 10:1176–1196

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bock O, Abeele S, Eversheim U (2003) Human adaptation to rotated vision: interplay of a continuous and a discrete process. Exp Brain Res 152:528–532

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos AP, Kettner RE, Schwartz AB (1988) Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three-dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neuronal population. J Neurosci 8:2928–2937

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ghahramani Z, Wolpert D (1997) Modular decomposition in visuomotor learning. Nature 386:392–395

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Imamizu H, Uno Y, Kawato M (1995) Internal representations of the motor apparatus: implications from generalization n visuomotor learning. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:1174–1198

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Inoue K, Kawashima R, Satoh K, Kinomura S, Sugiura M, Goto R, Ito M, Fukuda H (2000) A PET study of visuomotor learning under optical rotation. NeuroImage 11:505–516

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kalaska JF, Caminiti R, Georgopoulos AP (1983) Cortical mechanisms related to the direction of two-dimensional arm movements: relations in parietal area 5 and comparison with motor cortex. Exp Brain Res 51:247–260

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krakauer J, Pine Z, Ghilardi M-F, Ghez C (2000) Learning of visuomotor transformations for vectorial planning of reaching trajectories. J Neurosci 20:8916–8924

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lee J, Schweighofer N (2009) Dual adaptation supports a parallel architecture of motor memory. J Neurosci 29:10396–10404

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ottes F, Van Gibergen J, Eggermont J (1986) Visuomotor fields of the superior colliculus: a quantitative model. Vision Res 26:857–873

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Roby-Brami A, Burnod Y (1995) Learning a new visuomotor transformation: error correction and generalization. Cogn Brain Res 2:229–242

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith M, Ghazizadeh A, Shadmehr R (2006) Interacting adaptive processes with different timescales underlie short-term motor learning. PLoS Biol 4:1035–1043

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Snoddy G (1926) Learning and stability. J Appl Psychol 10:1–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stratton S, Liu Y-T, Hong S, Mayer-Kress G, Newell K (2007) Snoddy (1926) revisited: time scales of motor learning. J Motor Behav 39:503–515

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka H, Sejnowski T, Krakauer J (2009) Adaptation to visuomotor rotation through interaction between posterior parietal and motor cortical areas. J Neurophysiol 102:2921–2932

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang J, Sainburg R (2005) Adaptation to visuomotor rotations remaps movement vectors, not final positions. J Neurosci 25:4024–4030

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Woolley D, Tresilian J, Carson R, Rick S (2007) Dual adaptation to two opposing visuomotor rotations when each is associated with different regions of workspace. Exp Brain Res 179:155–165

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the German Space Agency DLR on behalf of the Bundesministeriu für Wirtschaft und Technologie (Grant 50WB0625). Thanks are due to Dipl.-Ing. (FH) L. Geisen for software development and to V. Rohde, K. Friederichs, and M. Hinrichs for their help with data collection and analysis.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Otmar Bock.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bock, O., Schmitz, G. Adaptation to rotated visual feedback depends on the number and spread of target directions. Exp Brain Res 209, 409–413 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2564-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2564-8

Keywords

Navigation