Skip to main content
Log in

Motor equivalence and self-motion induced by different movement speeds

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated pointing movements in 3D asking two questions: (1) Is goal-directed reaching accompanied by self-motion, a component of the joint velocity vector that leaves the hand’s movement unaffected? (2) Are differences in the terminal joint configurations among different speeds of reaching motor equivalent (i.e., terminal joint configurations differ more in directions of joint space that do not produce different pointer-tip positions than in directions that do) or non-motor equivalent (i.e., terminal joint configurations differ equally or more in directions of joint space that lead to different pointer-tip positions than in directions that do not affect the pointer-tip position). Subjects reached from an identical starting joint configuration and pointer-tip location to targets at slow, moderate, and fast speeds. Ten degrees of freedom of joint motion of the arm were recorded. The relationship between changes in the joint configuration and the three-dimensional pointer-tip position was expressed by a standard kinematic model, and the range- and null subspaces were computed from the associated Jacobian matrix. (1) The joint velocity vector and (2) the difference vector between terminal joint configurations from pairs of speed conditions were projected into the two subspaces. The relative length of the two components was used to quantify the amount of self-motion and the presence of motor equivalence, respectively. Results revealed that reaches were accompanied by a significant amount of self-motion at all reaching speeds. Self-motion scaled with movement speed. In addition, the difference in the terminal joint configuration between pairs of different reaching speeds revealed motor equivalence. The results are consistent with a control system that takes advantage of motor redundancy, allowing for flexibility in the face of perturbations, here induced by different movement speeds.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Atkeson CG, Hollerbach JM (1985) Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements. J Neurosci 5:2318–2330

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Belongie S (1999) Rodrigues’ rotation formula. In: Weisstein EW (ed). MathWorld–A Wolfram Web Resource

  • Bernstein N (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Pergamon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole KJ, Abbs JH (1986) Coordination of three-joint digit movements for rapid finger-thumb grasp. J Neurophysiol 55:1407–1423

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cole KJ, Abbs JH (1987) Kinematic and electromyographic responses to perturbation of a rapid grasp. J Neurophysiol 57:1498–1510

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cruse H, Bruwer M, Dean J (1993) Control of three- and four-joint arm movement: strategies for a manipulator with redundant degrees of freedom. J Mot Behav 25:131–139

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget M, Prablanc C (1997) Postural control of three-dimensional prehension movements. J Neurophysiol 77:452–464

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget M, Prablanc C, Rossetti Y, Arzi M, Paulignan Y, Urquizar C, Mignot JC (1995) Postural and synergic control for three-dimensional movements of reaching and grasping. J Neurophysiol 74:905–910

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget M, Gréa H, Prablanc C (1998) Final posture of the upper limb depends on the initial position of the hand during prehension movements. Exp Brain Res 119:511–516

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gelfand IM, Latash ML (1998) On the problem of adequate language in motor control. Mot Control 2:306–313

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gréa H, Desmurget M, Prablanc C (2000) Postural invariance in three-dimensional reaching and grasping movements. Exp Brain Res 134:155–162

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollerbach JM, Flash T (1982) Dynamic interactions between limb segments during planar arm movement. Biol Cybern 44:67–77

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JA, Tuller B, Vatikiotis-Bateson E, Fowler CA (1984) Functionally specific articulatory cooperation following jaw perturbations during speech: evidence for coordinative structures. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10:812–832

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klein CA, Huang C (1983) Review of pseudoinverse control for use with kinematically redundant manipulators. IEEE Trans Syst Man Cybern 13:245–250

    Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML, Scholz JP, Schoner G (2007) Toward a new theory of motor synergies. Mot Control 11:276–308

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin V, Scholz JP, Schöner G (2009) Redundancy, self-motion, and motor control. Neural Comput 21:1371–1414

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Murray R, Li Z, Sastry SS (1994) A mathematical introduction to robotic manipulation. CRC Press, Boca Raton

    Google Scholar 

  • Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Hogan N (1991) Integrable solutions of kinematic redundancy via impedance control. Int J Robot Res 10:481–491

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nishikawa KC, Murray ST, Flanders M (1999) Do arm postures vary with the speed of reaching? J Neurophysiol 81:2582–2586

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reisman DS, Scholz JP (2003) Aspects of joint coordination are preserved during pointing in persons with post-stroke hemiparesis. Brain 126:2510–2527

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum DA, Meulenbroek RGJ, Vaughan J (1999) Remembered positions: stored locations or stored postures? Exp Brain Res 124:503–512

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum DA, Meulenbroek RG, Vaughan J (2001a) Planning reaching and grasping movements: theoretical premises and practical implications. Mot Control 5:99–115

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum DA, Meulenbroek RJ, Vaughan J, Jansen C (2001b) Posture-based motion planning: applications to grasping. Psychol Rev 108:709–734

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum DA, Cohen RG, Dawson AM, Jax SA, Meulenbroek RG, van der Wel R, Vaughan J (2009) The posture-based motion planning framework: new findings related to object manipulation, moving around obstacles, moving in three spatial dimensions, and haptic tracking. Adv Exp Med Biol 629:485–497

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sainburg RL, Kalakanis D (2000) Differences in control of limb dynamics during dominant and nondominant arm reaching. J Neurophysiol 83:2661–2675

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz JP, Schoner G (1999) The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task. Exp Brain Res 126:289–306

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz JP, Schoner G, Latash ML (2000) Identifying the control structure of multijoint coordination during pistol shooting. Exp Brain Res 135:382–404

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz JP, Schöner G, Hsu WL, Jeka JJ, Horak FB, Martin V (2007) Motor equivalent control of the center of mass in response to support surface perturbations. Exp Brain Res 180:163–179

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schöner G (1995) Recent developments and problems in human movement science and their conceptual implications. Ecol Psychol 7:291–314

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soderkvist I, Wedin PA (1993) Determining the movements of the skeleton using well-configured markers. J Biomech 26:1473–1477

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Soechting JF, Lacquaniti F (1981) Invariant characteristics of a pointing movement in man. J Neurosci 1:710–720

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Soechting JF, Buneo CA, Herrmann U, Flanders M (1995) Moving effortlessly in three dimensions: does Donder’s law apply to arm movement? J Neurosci 15:6271–6280

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas JS, Corcos DM, Hasan Z (2003) Effect of movement speed on limb segment motions for reaching from a standing position. Exp Brain Res 148:377–387

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tillery SI, Ebner TJ, Soechting JF (1995) Task dependence of primate arm postures. Exp Brain Res 104:1–11

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Todorov E (2004) Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nat Neurosci 7:907–915

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Torres EB, Andersen R (2006) Space-time separation during obstacle-avoidance learning in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 96:2613–2632

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Torres EB, Zipser D (2002) Reaching to grasp with a multi-jointed arm. I. A computational model. J Neurophysiol 88:1–13

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torres EB, Zipser D (2004) Simultaneous control of hand displacements and rotations in orientation-matching experiments. J Appl Physiol 96:1978–1987

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tseng Y, Scholz JP (2005) The effect of workspace on the use of motor abundance. Motor Control 9

  • Tseng Y, Scholz JP, Schöner G (2002) Goal-equivalent joint coordination in pointing: effect of vision and arm dominance. Mot Control 6:183–207

    Google Scholar 

  • Tseng Y, Scholz JP, Schöner G, Hotchkiss L (2003) Effect of accuracy constraint on joint coordination during pointing movements. Exp Brain Res 149:276–288

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uno Y, Kawato M, Suzuki R (1989) Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement. Minimum torque-change model. Biol Cybern 61:89–101

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yang JF, Scholz JP (2005) Learning a throwing task is associated with differential changes in the use of motor abundance. Exp Brain Res 163:137–158

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NINDS Grant R01-NS050880, awarded to John Scholz.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. P. Scholz.

Appendix

Appendix

Self-motion related to the three-dimensional movement of the hand refers to a component of the joint velocity vector that does not affect hand motion (Murray et al. 1994). Self-motion is only possible if joint motions are redundant. Although self-motion could be considered wasted motion with respect to movement efficiency, the ability to produce self-motion is crucial for the performance of multiple tasks simultaneously. For example, the redundancy of angular motion of arm joints allows one to flip a light switch with the elbow while carrying and stabilizing a tray of drinks, i.e., without negatively impacting the hand’s movement. Thus, the component of the joint velocities linked to flipping the switch is self-motion when viewed with respect to hand movement. In principle, without a specific secondary task as was the case in the current experiment, one could expect self-motion to be minimal, most of the joint velocity vector acting to move the hand in space.

The conceptual framework of the method used here to differentiate between the component of the joint velocity vector that leads to hand movement, referred to by its technical term range-space motion (Murray et al. 1994), and the self-motion component is based on the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) variance analysis (Scholz and Schoner 1999). However, this analysis does not examine variances. The method can be illustrated by a simple example. Consider pointing with the hand in the horizontal plane where only three joint angles are available to transport the hand, shoulder horizontal flexion–extension, elbow flexion–extension, and wrist flexion–extension. To reach a target, at least two DOFs of joint motion are required, equivalent to the required two-dimensional hand position. Thus, there is one free joint DOF in this redundant system. That is, there exists a one-dimensional subspace (Fig. 5) within the space of the three arm joint motions within which a set of different combinations of shoulder, elbow, and wrist angles lead to an identical two-dimensional hand position. This subspace has been referred to as the uncontrolled manifold (UCM; Scholz and Schoner 1999) and a different UCM exists at each time point due to changing arm geometry. Thus, the first step in the self-motion analysis, like UCM analysis, was to estimate the UCM subspace at each data sample. First, the geometric model relating joint motion to hand motion is obtained as follows for the illustration:

$$ \left[ {\begin{array}{*{20}c} x \\ y \\ \end{array} } \right] = \left[ {\begin{array}{*{20}c} {l_{\text{arm}} *\cos \left( {\theta_{\text{shoulder}} } \right) + l_{\text{forearm}} *\cos \left( {\theta_{\text{shoulder}} + \theta_{\text{elbow}} } \right) + l_{\text{hand}} *\cos \left( {\theta_{\text{shoulder}} + \theta_{\text{elbow}} + \theta_{\text{wrist}} } \right)} \\ {l_{\text{arm}} *\sin \left( {\theta_{\text{shoulder}} } \right) + l_{\text{forearm}} *\sin \left( {\theta_{\text{shoulder}} + \theta_{\text{elbow}} } \right) + l_{\text{hand}} *\sin \left( {\theta_{\text{shoulder}} + \theta_{\text{elbow}} + \theta_{\text{wrist}} } \right)} \\ \end{array} } \right]. $$

The Jacobian (J) of the geometric model is then computed using each instantaneous joint configuration ‘i’, [θ ishoulder , θ ielbow , θ iwrist ]T, of each trial. The Jacobian is defined as the matrix of partial derivatives of the hand coordinates relative to the joint angles,

$$ J(\theta ) = \left[{\begin{array}{*{20}c} {\partial x/\partial \theta_{\text{shoulder}} \;\partial x/\partial \theta_{\text{elbow}} \;\partial x/\partial \theta_{\text{wrist}} } \\ {\partial y/\partial \theta_{\text{shoulder}} \;\partial y/\partial \theta_{\text{elbow}} \;\partial x/\partial \theta_{\text{wrist}} } \\ \end{array}}\right ]$$

.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Cartoon examples of self-motion and range-space motion estimates from linear estimates of uncontrolled manifolds (UCMs). JC = joint configurations; Upper panel shows an example where there would be a relatively large amount of self-motion. The lower panel is an example of low self-motion. See “Appendix” for details

The Jacobian is used to transform changes in joint angles or angular velocities into hand or more generally end-effector velocities, i.e., dr/dt = J(θ) * dθ/dt.

The null space of the Jacobian is then computed by singular value decomposition of the Jacobian in Matlab™, [u, s, v] = svd(JT), where ‘T’ is the transpose of the Jacobian, ‘u’ is a matrix of eigenvectors, and ‘s’ is the diagonal matrix of eigenvalues. The first two columns of u are the range-space vectors, and the last column is the null-space vector in this 3D example.

The final step in the self-motion analysis is to project the instantaneous joint velocity vector [dθ ishoulder /dt, dθ ielbow /dt, dθ iwrist /dt]T into the range space and null space and then compute the lengths of projection.

Figure 5 provides a cartoon illustration of the results of this procedure. In the figure, three hypothetical one-dimensional UCMs representing three consecutive time points are illustrated. The solid filled circles lying on each UCM represent joint angle combinations, or joint configurations, for a given trial at each time point. Note that given the definition of the UCM, combinations of joints lying anywhere along each line would produce the same 2D hand position (but a different hand position for each UCM). Thus, movement of the hand can be conceived of as a transition among sequences of UCMs. The heavy dashed line in each panel represents the change in joint configuration, or the joint velocity vector. If most of the joint velocity vectors act to move the hand in space, then this vector should be relatively perpendicular to the UCMs. If, instead, there is a significant amount of self-motion, then the vector will form a more acute angle with the UCMs. The upper panel is an illustration of the later situation. The lower panel represents a hand movement accompanied by little self-motion. The different situations are estimated by the lengths of projection of the joint velocity vector into each subspace, i.e., the null space (self-motion) and its complement (range-space motion). These projection lengths are also illustrated in the figure.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Scholz, J.P., Dwight-Higgin, T., Lynch, J.E. et al. Motor equivalence and self-motion induced by different movement speeds. Exp Brain Res 209, 319–332 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2541-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2541-2

Keywords

Navigation