Skip to main content
Log in

Latent state-trait models in attitude research

  • Published:
Quality and Quantity Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We propose that a distinction be made between attitude states and traits. Attitudes are never measured in a situational vacuum. The situation in which attitudes are assessed and the interaction between person and situation may have systematic effects on answers to attitude questionnaires. Nevertheless, systematic and stable interindividual differences also exist. We supplement the classical concept of reliability by two coefficients: the consistency coefficient is the proportion of variance of an observed variable due to interindividual differences. The specificity coefficient is the proportion of variance due to (a) the different situations that may occur for different persons at an occasion of measurement, and (b) the person-situation interaction. The sum of both coefficients is the reliability coefficient for the occasion of measurement considered. A class of models consisting of assumptions similar to the classical assumptions of essential τ-equivalence are illustrated. These models imply special confirmatory factor models. Hence, parameter estimation and hypothesis testing may be accomplished by LISREL (Jöreskog and Sörbom, 1988), EQS (Bentler, 1985), or LISCOMP (Muthén, 1988). The models are illustrated by a reanalysis of some data on attitudes toward guestworkers (Porst and Zeifang, 1987).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bem D. J. (1972). “Self-perception theory”, in L.Berkowitz (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Vol. 6. New York: Academic Press, pp. 1–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentler P. M. (1985). Theory and Implementation of EQS, a Structural Equation Program. Los Angeles, CA: BMDP Statistical Software.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boosch A. (1986). Attitüden und Pseudoattitüden. Frankfurt: Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell D. T. (1950). “The indirect assessment of social attitudes”, Psychological Bulletin 47: 15–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell R. B., Cattell A. K., and Rhymer R. M. (1947). “P-technique demonstrated in determining psychophysiological source traits in a normal individual”, Psychometrika 12: 267–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell R. B. and Scheier I. H. (1961). The Meaning and Measurement of Neuroticism and Anxiety. New York: Ronald Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach L. J., Gleser G. C., Nanda H., and Rajaratnam N. (1972). The Dependability of Behavioral Measurements. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein S. (1977). “Traits are alive and well”, in D.Magnusson, and N. S.Endler (eds.), Personality at the Crossroads: Current Issues in Interactional Psychology. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum, pp. 209–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein S. (1979). “The stability of behavior: I. On predicting most of the people much of the time”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37: 1097–1126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein S. (1980). “The stability of behavior: II. Implications for psychological research”, American Psychologist 35: 790–806.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein S. (1990). “Comment on the effects of aggregation across and within occasions on consistency, specificity and reliability”, Methodika 4: 95–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein S. and O'Brien E. J. (1985). “The person-situation debate in historical and current perspective”, Psychological Bulletin 98: 513–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green B. F. (1954). “Attitude measurement”, in G.Lindzey (ed.), Handbook of Social Psychology. Vol. 1. Reading, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley, pp. 335–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green B. F. (1969). Attitude Measurement. Reading, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog C. and Nesselroade J. R. (1987). “Beyond autoregressive models: Some implications of the trait-state distinction for the structural modeling of developmental change”, Child Development 58: 93–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jogodzinski W., Kühnel S. M., and Schmidt P. (1987). “Is there a “Socratic Effect” in nonexperimental panel studies?”, Sociological Methods and Research 15: 259–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jöreskog K. G. and Sörbom D. (1988). LISREL 7: A Guide to the Program and Applications. Chicago: SPSS Incorporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch J.-J. (1974). “Elaboration von Attitüden aus Anlaß ihrer Messung: Präsentation eines Problems”, in L. H.Eckensberger and U. S.Eckensberger (eds.), Bericht über den 28. Kongreß der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie in Saarbrücken. Göttingen: Hogrefe, pp. 271–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krech D. and Crutchfield R. S. (1948). Theory and Problems of Social Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire W. J. (1960). “A syllogistic analysis of cognitive relationships”, in M. J.Rosenberg, C. I.Hovland, W. J.McGuire, R. P.Abelson, and J. W.Brehm (eds.), Attitude Organization and Change. Westport, Connecticut: Greenwood Press, pp. 65–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel W. (1968). Personality and Assessment. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mummendey H. D. (ed.) (1988). Verhalten und Einstellung. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muthén B. (1983). “Latent variable structural equation modeling with categorical data”, Journal of Econometrics 22: 43–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muthén B. (1988). LISCOMP: Analysis of Linear Structural Equations with a Comprehensive Measurement Model (2nd edn.). Mooresville, Indiana: Scientific Software.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paunonen S. V. (1984). “The reliability of aggregated measurements: Lessons to be learned from psychometric theory”, Journal of Research in Personality 18: 383–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty R. E., Ostrom T. M., and Brock T. C. (eds.) (1981). Cognitive Responses in Persuasion. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porst R. and Zeifang K. (1987). “A description of the German General Social Survey Test-Retest Study and a report on the stabilities of the sociodemographic variables”, Sociological Methods and Research 15: 177–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rushton J. P., Brainerd C. J., and Pressley M. (1983). “Behavioral development and construct validity: The principle of aggregation”, Psychological Bulletin 94: 18–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saris W. and van den Putte B. (1987). “True score or factor models: A secondary analysis of the ALLBUS-Test-Retest Data”, Sociological Methods and Research 17: 123–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlegel R. P. and Ditecco D. (1982). “Attitudinal structures and attitude-behavior relation”, in M. P.Zanna, E. T.Higgins, and C. P.Herman (eds.), Consistency in Social Behavior. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum, pp. 17–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt M. J. and Steyer R. (1990). “Beyond intuition and classical test theory: A reply to Epstein”, Methodika 4: 101–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger C. D. (1972). “Anxiety as an emotional state”, in C. D.Spielberger (ed.), Anxiety: Current Trends in Theory and Research. Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press, pp. 23–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steyer R. (1987). “Konsistenz und Spezifität: Definition zweier zentraler Begriffe der Differentiellen Psychologie und ein einfaches Modell zu ihrer Identifikation”, Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie 8: 245–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steyer R. (1988). Experiment, Regression und Kausalität: Die logische Struktur kausaler Regressionsmodelle. Trier: Universität Trier. Unveröffentlichte Habilitationsschrift.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steyer R. (1989). “Models of classical psychometric test theory as stochastic models: Representation, uniqueness, meaningfulness, identifiability, and testability”, Methodika 3: 25–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steyer R. and SchmittM. J. (1990). “The effects of aggregation across and within occasions on consistency, specificity, and reliability”, Methodika 4: 58–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tack W. H. (1980). “Zur Theorie psychometrischer Verfahren. Formalisierung der Erfassung von Situationsabhängigkeit und Veränderung”, Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie 1: 87–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tesser A. (1978). “Self-generated attitude change”, in L.Berkowitz (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Vol. 11. New York: Academic Press, pp. 289–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorne F. C. (1966). “Theory of the psychological state”, Journal of Clinical Psychology 22: 127–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman D. W. (1975). “Probability spaces, Hilbert spaces, and the axioms of test theory”, Psychometrika 40: 395–412.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Steyer, R., Schmitt, M.J. Latent state-trait models in attitude research. Qual Quant 24, 427–445 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00152014

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00152014

Keywords

Navigation