Skip to main content
Log in

Jealousy, general creativity, and coping with social frustration during the menstrual cycle

  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Sensitivity to feelings of jealousy in women (n=16) at three phases of their menstrual cycle differing in serum hormone concentrations (menses, preovulatory phase, midluteal phase) was investigated. Nonspecific electrodermal activity indicating sympathetic excitation was measured while subjects listened to stories and imagined situations inducing (i) nonsexual jealousy (the partner forms a deep emotional attachment to another woman), (ii) sexual jealousy (the partner has sexual intercourse with another woman), and (iii) no jealousy (social interaction lacking intense emotionality). Subjective ratings of the intensity of jealousy during both stories of jealousy were also assessed. In addition, changes in cognitive coping abilities, in general, and in the context of socially frustrating events, were assessed by tests of divergent thinking (creativity) and the Rosenzweig Picture Frustration Test. Frequency of nonspecific electrodermal reactions was generally enhanced during the preovulatory phase. This increase was particularly strong when the women listened to stories inducing nonsexual jealousy, i.e., a story characterized by cues of a complete loss of the partner's investment. Rated jealousy tended to be enhanced during the ovulatory phase, especially after the story of nonsexual jealousy. These changes in emotionality were paralleled by an improved divergent thinking performance and less aggressive coping with socially frustrating events around the time of ovulation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Asso, D. (1986). Psychology degree examinations and premenstrual phase.Women Health 10: 91–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bancroft, J. (1984). Hormones and human sexual behavior.J. Sex Marital Ther. 10: 3–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, D., Creutzfeld, O. D., and Wuttke, W. (1980). Electrophysiological and psychological changes induced by steroid hormones in men and women.Acta Psychiat. Belg. 80: 674–697.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkovec, T. D. (1976). Physiological and cognitive processes in the regulation of anxiety. In Schwartz, G. E., and Shapiro, D. (eds.),Consciousness and Self-Regulation, Vol. 1, Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boucsein, W. (1988).Elektrodermale Aktivität: Grundlagen, Methoden und Anwendungen, Springer, Berlin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broverman, D. M., Vogel, W., Klaiber, E. L., Majcher, D., Shea, D., and Paul, V. (1981). Changes in cognitive task performance across the menstrual cycle.Physiol. Psychol. 95: 646–654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunel, U. (1991). Cognition, memory and the menstrual cycle.Cah. Psychol. Cognit. 11: 3–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M., Larsen, J. L., Semmelroth, J., and Westen, D. (1992). Sex differences in jealousy: evolution, physiology, and psychology.Psychol. Sci. 3: 251–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowley, W. R., O'Donohue, T. L., Wachslight, H., and Jacobowitz, D. M. (1978). Effect of estrogens and progesterone on plasma gonadotropins and on catecholamine levels and turnover in discrete brain regions of ovariectomized rats.Brain Res. 154: 345–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, J. M. (1964). Measuring creative abilities in public relations and in advertising work. In Tylor, C. W. (ed.),Widening Horizons in Creativity, Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S., and Fenz, W. D. (1966). Adaptation of the GSR and basal conductance as a function of manifest anxiety. Unpublished mimeograph.

  • Gold, A., and Adams, D. B. (1981). Motivational factors affecting fluctuations of female sexual activity at menstruation.Psychol. Women Quart. 5: 670–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gomez-Amor, J., Martinez-Selva, J. M., Roman, F., and Zamora, S. (1990). Electrodermal activity, hormonal levels and subjective experience during the menstrual cycle.Biol. Psychol. 30: 125–139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, H. W., and Lee, P. A. (1993). No difference in cognitive performance between phases of the menstrual cycle.Psychoneuroendocrinology 18: 521–531.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, E. A. (1980). Cognition as related to menstrual cycle phase and estrogen level. In Dan, A. J., Graham, E. A., and Beecher, C. (eds.),The Menstrual Cycle: A Synthesis of Interdisciplinary Research, Springer, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, E. A., Glasser, M. (1985). Relationship of pregnanediol level to cognitive behavior and mood.Psychosom. Med. 47: 26–34.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1967).The Nature of Human Intelligence. McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P., and Hoepfner, R. (1971).The Analysis of Intelligence, McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hampson, E. (1990a). Estrogen related variations in human spatial and articulatory-motor skills.Psychoneuroendocrinology 15: 97–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hampson, E. (1990b). Variations in sex-related cognitive abilities across the menstrual cycle.Brain Cognit. 14: 26–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, S. M. (1987). Female sexual behavior: Fluctuations during the menstrual cycle.J. Psychosom. Res. 31: 101–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Inaba, M., and Kamata K. (1979). Effect of estradiol-17 beta and other steroids on noradrenaline and dopamine binding to synaptic membrane fragments of rat brain.J. Steroid Biochem. 11: 1491–1497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., and Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving.J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52: 1122–1131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, D. A., and Narayana, K. (1978). Effects of oestradiol-17 beta on monoamine concentrations in the hypothalamus of anoestrous ewe.J. Physiol. 282: 44–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krug, R., Stamm, U., Pietrowsky, R., Fehm, H. L., and Born, J. (1994). Effects of menstrual cycle on creativity.Psychoneuroendocrinology 19: 21–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacey, J. I. (1956). The evaluation of automatic responses: Toward a general solution.Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 67: 125–163.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mainberger, U. (1977).Test zum divergenten Denken (Kreativität) TDK 4-6, Beltz, Weinheim, Germany.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G. (1986). The interactive effects of extraversion and arousal on performance: Are creativity tests anomalous?Pers. Indiv. Diff. 7: 751–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martinez-Selva, J. M., Gomez-Amor, J., Olmos, E., Navarro, N., and Roman, F. (1987). Sex and menstrual cycle differences in the habituation and spontaneous recovery of the electrodermal orienting reaction.Pers. Indiv. Diff. 8: 211–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matteo, S., and Rissman, E. F. (1984). Increased sexual activity during the midcycle portion of the human menstrual cycle.Horm. Behav. 18: 249–255.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, N. M., Udry, J. R., Khan, F., and Dawood, M. Y. (1987). Marital sex frequency and midcycle female testosterone.Arch. Sex. Behav. 16: 27–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pollack, J. (1989). Animal Behavior, Sienauer Associates, Thunderland, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rauchfleisch, U. (1987). Pilot-Studie zur Entwicklung von zwei äquivalenten Halbformen des Rosenzweig Picture-Frustration Tests.Diagnostica 33: 354–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, E. J. (1980). The relationship between hormonal changes and psychophysiological measures in women.Biol. Psychol. Bull. 6: 46–56.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, S. (1950). Manual for the Rosenzweig Picture-Frustration Study, Adult Form, privately published, St. Louis, MO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, S., and Adelmann, S. (1977). Construct validity of the Rosenzweig Picture-Frustration Study.J. Pers. Assess. 41: 578–588.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, S., and Rosenzweig, L. (1976). Guide to research on the Rosenzweig Picture-Frustration Study, 1934–1974.J. Pers. Assess. 40: 599–606.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P., and Johnston, J. C. (1973). A cognitive theory of avoidance learning. In McGuigan, F. J., and Lumsden, D. B. (eds.),Contemporary Approaches to Conditioning and Learning, Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherwin, B. B., and Gelfand, M. M. (1985). Sex steroids and affect in the surgical menopause: A double-blind, cross-over study.Psychoneuroendocrinology 10: 325–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sherwin, B. B., and Gelfand, M. M. (1987). The role of androgen in the maintenance of sexual functioning in oophorectomized women.Psychosom. Med. 49: 397–409.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, A. J., Cohen, S. I., and Shmavonian, B. M. (1959). Investigation of psychophysiological relationships with skin resistance measures.J. Psychosom. Res. 4: 65–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, B. A. (1983). How does menstruation affect cognitive competence and psychophysiological response?Women Health 8: 53–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uno, T. (1973). GSR activity and the human menstrual cycle.Psychophysiology 10: 213–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, M. A., and Kogan, N. (1969).Modes of Thinking in Young Children, Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Research supported by a grant from the Deütsche Forschüngs Gemeinschaft to J. Born.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Krug, R., Finn, M., Pietrowsky, R. et al. Jealousy, general creativity, and coping with social frustration during the menstrual cycle. Arch Sex Behav 25, 181–199 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02437935

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02437935

Key words

Navigation