Skip to main content

The Linear Logistic Test Model and heterogeneity of cognitive strategies

  • Chapter
Mathematical Psychology in Progress

Part of the book series: Recent Research in Psychology ((PSYCHOLOGY))

Abstract

Some properties of the Linear Logistic Test Model (LLTM) are discussed. Two prerequisites should be met in order to test a cognitive model by means of the LLTM. First, the items used in testing the cognitive model should make up a Rasch homogeneous scale. Second, the population under consideration should be homogeneous with regard to the cognitive strategy employed in solving items representing the task at hand. For a task consisting of solving balance problems it is demonstrated that the second prerequisite is not fulfilled As a consequence the LLTM does not fit for the whole population. By dividing the population into four strategy homogeneous subpopulations a fitting LLTM could be found within one of these subpopulations. Consequently, it is recommended that in using the LLTM for testing cognitive models the population under consideration should be investigated with respect to different cognitive strategies.

This is a modified version of a paper earlier published in Dutch: Het Lineair Logistisch Test Model en heterogeniteit van cognitieve strategieën bij balansproblemen. Tijdschrift voor onderwijsresearch 13(1988), 301–310.

Bitnet/EARN: LVMAANEN @ HGRRUG5, UUCP: {... ! mcvax !} guvaxin ! guicce ! leo

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Andersen, E.B. (1973). A goodness of fit test for the Rasch model. Psychometrika, 38, 123–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. (1978). Intelligence and general strategies. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies in Information Processing. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Been, P.H., Jorna, R.J. & Sijtsma, K. (1984). Over kijken, meten en modellen bij breukrekentaken. In P.G. Vos, K.B. Koster Sc J. Kingma (Eds.). Rekenen. Balans van standpunten in theorievorming en empirisch onderzoek. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergan, J.R., Towstopiat, O., Cancelli, A.A. & Karp, C. (1982). Replacement and component rules in hierarchically ordered mathematics rule learning tasks. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 32–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D.E. (1984). The Maltese cross, a new simplistic model for memory. The Behavioral and’ Brain Sciences, 7, 55–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J.S. & VanLehn, K. (1980). Repair theory: a generative theory of bugs in procedural skills. Cognitive Science, 2, 379–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterfield, E.C. & Belmont, J.M. (1971). Relations of storage and retrieval strategies as short-term memory processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 319–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, L.A. (1982). Strategies for Visual Comparison and Representation: Individual Differences. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.). Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence. Hillsdale (N.J.): Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldt, L.S. (1965). The approximate sampling distribution of Kuder-Richardson reliability coefficient twenty. Psychometrika, 30, 357–370.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, G.H. (1973). The linear logistic test model as an instrument in educational research. Acta Psychologica, 37, 359–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, G.H. (1974). Einfiihrung in die Theorie psychologischer Tests. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, G.H. (1983). Logistic latent trait models with linear constraints. Psychometrika, 48, 3–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, G.H. & Formann, A.K. (1982). Some applications of logistic latent trait models with linear constraints on the parameters. Applied Psychological Measurement, 6, 397–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Groen, G.J. & Parkman, J.M. (1972). A chronometric analysis of simple addition, Psychological Review, 72, 329–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haeussler, P. (1977). Investigation of mathematical reasoning in science problems. In H. Spada & W.F. Kempf (Eds.). Structural Models of Thinking and Learning. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E.B., Frost, N. & Lunneborg, C.L. (1973). Individual differences in cognition: A new approach to intelligence. In G. Bower (Ed.). The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E.B., (1974). Quote the Raven? Nevermore! In L. Gregg (Ed.). Knowledge and Cognition. Hillsdale (N.J.): Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubinger, K.D. (1979). Das Problemlöseverhalten bei der statistischen Auswertung psychologischer Experimente. Ein Beispiel hochschuldidaktischer Forschung. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 26, 467–495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubinger, K.D. (1980). Die Anwendung von Fischers linear logistischen Test Modell zur Leistungssteigerung in Lehrveranstaltungen — Einige neuere Ergebnisse. Archiv für Psychologie, 133, 69–79.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, C. (1981). Skill in Algebra. In J.R. Anderson (Ed.). Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Hillsdale (N.J.): Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C.M., Hunt, E.B. & Mathews, N.N. (1978). Individual differences in the verification of sentence-picture relationships. Journal of Verbal Learning and Behavior, 17, 493–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nährer, W. (1980). Zur Analyse von Matrizenaufgaben mit dem linearen logistischen Testmodell. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 27, 553–564.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1973). You can’t play 20 questions with nature and win. In W.G. Chase (Ed.). Visual Information Processing. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheiblechner, H. (1972). Das Lernen und Lösen komplexer Denkaufgaben. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 19, 476–506.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R.S. (1976). Three aspects of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 481–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R.S. & Klahr, D. (1982). When do children learn? In R. Glaser (Ed.). Advances in Instructional Psychology, vol 2. Hillsdale (N.J.): Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sijtsma, K. (1982). Een lineair logistisch model ter verklaring van de moeilijkheidspa-rameters van breukrekenitems. In J.G.L.C. Lodewijks & P.R.J. Simons (Eds.). Strategieén in leren en ontwikkeling. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spada, H. (1976). Modelle des Denkens und Lernens. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R.J. & Weil, E.M. (1980). An Aptitude x Strategy Interaction in Linear Syllogistic Reasoning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 226–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Svenson, O. & Hedenborg, M.L. (1980). Strategies used by children when solving simple subtractions. Acta Psychologica, 4, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, G. (1978). Strategies of Information Processing. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van de Vijver, F.J.R. (1988). Systematizing the item content in test design. In R. Langeheine & J. Rost (Eds.). Latent Trait and Latent Class Models. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitely, S.E. & Schneider, L.M. (1981). Information structure for geometric analo-gies: A test theory approach. Applied Psychological Measurement, 5, 383–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wishart, D. (1978). CL USTAN 1C. Utrecht: Centrum voor Data-Analyse, Rijksuniversiteit Utrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wollenberg, A.L. van den (1982). A simple and effective method to test the dimensionality axiom of the Rasch model. Applied Psychological Measurement, 6, 83–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, R. (1978). Fitting the Rasch model — A heady tale. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 31, 27–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, R.M. (1978). Strategies and the structure of cognitive skill. In G. Underwood (Ed.). Strategies of Information Processing. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwarts, M.A. (1983). Criteriumtoetsen bij aansluiting van primair en secundair onderwijs. (unpublished dissertation). Rijksuniversiteit Utrecht.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

van Maanen, L., Been, P., Sijtsma, K. (1989). The Linear Logistic Test Model and heterogeneity of cognitive strategies. In: Roskam, E.E. (eds) Mathematical Psychology in Progress. Recent Research in Psychology. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-83943-6_17

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-83943-6_17

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-540-51686-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-83943-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics