Skip to main content

A Construction Morphology Approach to Sign Language Analysis

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Construction of Words

Part of the book series: Studies in Morphology ((SUMO,volume 4))

Abstract

In this chapter, we extend a usage-based theory of Construction Morphology to the analysis of sign language structure, to address two long-standing categorization problems in sign language linguistics. Sign language linguistics traditionally distinguishes monomorphemic core lexical signs from multimorphemic classifier construction signs, based on whether or not a sign form exhibits analyzable morphological structure (“the Core vs. Classifier problem”). In this tradition, core signs are retrieved from the lexicon, while classifier signs are derived productively via grammatical rules. Sign linguists are also accustomed to classifying discrete and listable aspects of sign structure as language, while aspects of signing that exhibit more holism or gradience are considered to be gesture (“the Language vs. Gesture problem”). These categories of core vs. classifier on the one hand and language vs. gesture on the other derive from a shared source: the assumption that linguistic forms are built up from discrete building blocks. Instead, we analyze multimodal usage events in terms of constructions, conventional patterns of meaning and form containing both fixed elements and variable slots and organized in a structured network. We argue that the Construction Morphology approach leads to a uniform analysis of core and classifier signs alike, without resorting to an a priori distinction between language and gesture.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    We are aware only of Johnston and Ferrara (2012), on the topic of phrasal idioms, Lepic (2016), on derivational morphology, and Wilcox and Occhino (2016), on pointing constructions, as articles that explicitly adopt a construction-theoretic view of sign language structure.

  2. 2.

    https://twitter.com/FTLifeArts/status/722397326287970304

  3. 3.

    Vogler and Metaxas (2001) estimate over 1 billion phoneme combinations in ASL, and Morgan (2016) estimates that the distinctive features in Kenyan Sign Language yield over 12 billion possible sign forms.

  4. 4.

    Here we are oversimplifying quite a bit. The “formational parameters” that are most standardly discussed in sign phonology are “handshape”, “movement”, and “location”. However, sign forms also differ according to the relationship between the two hands, including whether one or two hands are used, the orientation of the hand(s) relative to the body, and/or the hands’ orientation relative to each other. Some signs are also canonically formed with certain facial expressions or body postures. Here we focus on “the shape of the hands” (handshape) and “how the hands move” (movement + location) only, for ease of exposition. We refer readers to Sandler and Lillo-Martin (2006) for an accessible and comprehensive overview of sign language phonology and morphology from a generative perspective.

  5. 5.

    Sign language linguistics has developed its own idiosyncratic glossing conventions. English words in small-caps indicate single ASL sign forms, as do English multiple words joined by hyphens.

  6. 6.

    See Jackendoff (1975), Anderson (1992), and Bochner (1993) for further discussions of the notion of “economy” and the development of morphological theory in generative linguistics.

  7. 7.

    The rule-list fallacy is part of a larger set of false dichotomies that Langacker (2008: 13) refers to as “exclusionary fallacies”. These fallacies stem from creating predetermined, mutually exclusive alternatives when formulating questions related to categorization or membership.

  8. 8.

    The term “lexicalization” in signed language linguistics is typically used in a very restricted sense to refer to the process by which any internally-structured construction, whether classifier constructions, fingerspelled words, or multi-sign phrases, become phonologically reduced and non-compositional. In spoken language linguistics, in contrast, the term lexicalization generally refers to a constellation of processes including routinization, conventionalization, and institutionalization, while formal reduction is only one component of a larger process (e.g., Hohenhaus 2005).

  9. 9.

    https://www.signingsavvy.com/sign/nice+to+meet+you

  10. 10.

    Bybee (2001; 11) demonstrates that in English, syllabicity is lost in sequences of unstressed schwa + resonant more quickly in more frequent forms such as every or memory. Every is often reduced from three to two or even to one syllable /, /ɛɹi/, as opposed to forms such as mammary or homily, which are less frequent and typically retain three syllables. Such a process, however, does not preclude speakers from recognizing these phonetic variants as belonging to the same lexical construction.

  11. 11.

    Evidence for the psychological reality of this non-compositional morphological schema comes from the fact that the signs glossed as a unt and female-c ousin, which are borrowed lexical items from English (known as “initialized signs”, Lepic 2015, Padden 1998, with the underlined letter in the gloss indicating the particular handshape used to form the sign), joined this sign family at the time of their borrowing. Because they also denote “female family members”, these borrowed signs are formed to also be signed near the signer’s chin.

  12. 12.

    A more traditional, compositional analysis of the signs in Figs 7 and 8 would be that four morphemes, “chin-female”, “chin-communication”, “chin-negative”, and “chin-eating”, are homophonous independent formatives listed as meaningful morphemes in the lexicon. These formatives are then combined with handshapes and movements, which themselves must also contribute meaning as morphemes, to derive signs compositionally via derivational rules (see the “S-morphs” of Liddell & Johnson 1989 and the “ion-morphs” of Fernald and Napoli 2000). This, once again, is the rule/list fallacy at work. We consider this approach to be problematic because there is no principled way to determine how these formatives combine to create a whole sign without access to the meaning of the whole sign in the first place (a familiar dilemma in morphological analysis, see Blevins 2016 for detailed discussion).

    Fig. 7
    figure 7

    A family of ASL signs sharing an aspect of meaning and an aspect of form: signs for “female family members” are conventionally signed at the signer’s chin

  13. 13.

    Wilcox and Occhino (2016) refer to this type of representation as a phonological rather than morphological schema. In usage-based accounts, these formal divisions are not central to explanations of grammatical phenomena, so we will not insist that a schema must either be phonological or morphological in nature. Nevertheless, these labels can be useful in a more descriptive sense: a phonological schema can be thought of as an abstraction of a formal pattern among related words, a semantic schema as an abstraction of patterns of meaning among related words, and a morphological schema as a statement about systematic relationships between form and meaning schemas.

  14. 14.

    Note that there are still elements of the signal that are not described here: prosody and intonation, widely acknowledged to vary continuously in the vocal signal, thus exhibiting “gestural” properties, even in the spoken modality, are not described in detail here.

References

  • Abner, N., K. Cooperrider, and S. Goldin-Meadow. 2015. Gesture for linguists: A handy primer. Language and Linguistics Compass 9 (11): 437–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, S.R. 1992. A-morphous morphology. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Andrén, M. 2014. Multimodal constructions in children: Is the headshake part of language? Gesture 14 (2): 141–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anible, B., and C. Occhino-Kehoe. 2014. What’s happening with HAPPEN: The grammaticalization of HAPPEN in American Sign Language. In Proceedings of the High Desert Linguistics Society Conference, ed. B. Anible, K. Beers, L. Hirrel, and D. Wager, 27–41. Albuquerque. Retrieved from http://linggraduate.unm.edu/HDLS10Proceedings.html.

  • Aronoff, M., I. Meir, C.A. Padden, and W. Sandler. 2003. Classifier constructions and morphology in two sign languages. In Perspectives on classifier constructions in signed languages, ed. K. Emmorey, 53–84. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L.W. 1999. Perceptual symbol systems. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (4): 577–609. discussion 610–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2008. Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59 (1): 617–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Battison, R. 1978. Lexical borrowing in American Sign Language. Silver Spring: Linstok Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayley, R., C. Lucas, and M. Rose. 2000. Variation in American sign language: The case of DEAF. Journal of Sociolinguistics 4: 81–107. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9481.00104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berent, I., and S. Goldin-Meadow. 2015. Language by mouth and by hand. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 78. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bergen, B. 2007. Experimental methods for simulation semantics. In Methods in cognitive linguistics, ed. M. Gonzalez-Marquez, I. Mittleberg, S. Coulson, and M.J. Spivey, 277–301. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blevins, J.P. 2016. Word and paradigm morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Blevins, J.P., F. Ackerman, and R. Malouf. 2016. Morphology as an adaptive discriminative system. In Morphological metatheory, ed. D. Siddiqi and H. Harley, 271–302. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bochner, H. 1993. Simplicity in generative morphology. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Booij, G. 2010. Construction morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2013. Morphology in construction grammar. In The Oxford handbook of construction grammar, ed. A. Bergs and G. Trousdale, 255–273. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2017. Inheritance and motivation in construction morphology. In Defaults in morphological theory, ed. N. Gisborne and A. Hippisley, 18–39. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brennan, M. 1990. Productive morphology in British Sign Language. Proceedings of the International Congress on Sign Language Research and Application, Hamburg, 205–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brentari, D. 1998. A prosodic model of sign language phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brentari, D., and C. Padden. 2001. A language with multiple origins: Native and foreign vocabulary in American Sign Language. In Foreign vocabulary in sign language: A crosslinguistic investigation of word formation, ed. D. Brentari, 87–119. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, P.J., M. Tomasello, K. Dodson, and L.B. Lewis. 1999. Young children’s overgeneralizations with fixed transitivity verbs. Child Development 70 (6): 1325–1337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J.L. 2001. Phonology and language use. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2006. From usage to grammar: The mind’s response to repetition. Language 82 (4): 711–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2010. Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J., and J. Scheibman. 1999. The effect of usage on degrees of constituency: The reduction of don’t in English. Linguistics 37 (4): 575–596. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.37.4.575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calbris, G. 1990. The semiotics of French gesture. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cienki, A. 2013. Image schemas and mimetic schemas in cognitive linguistics and gesture studies. Review of Cognitive Linguistics 11 (2): 417–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cormier, K., D. Quinto-Pozos, Z. Sevcikova, and A. Schembri. 2012. Lexicalisation and de-lexicalisation processes in sign languages: Comparing depicting constructions and viewpoint gestures. Language & Communication 32 (4): 329–348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langcom.2012.09.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cormier, K., S. Smith, and Z. Sevcikova. 2013. Predicate structures, gesture, and simultaneity in the representation of action in British Sign Language: Evidence from deaf children and adults. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 18 (3): 370–390. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/ent020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eccarius, P., and D. Brentari. 2007. Symmetry and dominance: A cross-linguistic study of signs and classifier constructions. The Linguistics of Sign Language Classifiers: Phonology, Morpho-Syntax, Semantics and Discourse. Lingua 117 (7): 1169–1201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2005.04.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J.L. 1995. Language as a dynamical system. In Mind as motion: Explorations in the dynamics of cognition, ed. R.F. Port and T. Van Gelder. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey, K. 1999. Do signers gesture? In Gesture, speech, and sign, ed. L. Messing and R. Campbell, 133–159. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fenlon, J., K. Cormier, and D. Brentari. 2018. The phonology of sign languages. In Handbook of phonological theory, ed. S.J. Hannahs and A. Bosch. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, T.B., and D.J. Napoli. 2000. Exploitation of morphological possibilities in signed languages comparison of American Sign Language with English. Sign Language & Linguistics 3 (1): 3–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C.J., P. Kay, and M.C. O’Connor. 1988. Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: The case of let alone. Language 64 (3): 501–538. https://doi.org/10.2307/414531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frishberg, N., and B. Gough. 2000. Morphology in American Sign Language. Sign Language & Linguistics 3 (1): 103–131. https://doi.org/10.1075/sll.3.1.08fri.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A.M., and M.P. Kaschak. 2002. Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 9 (3): 558–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A. 2006. Constructions at work: The nature of generalisations in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2013. Constructionist Approaches. In The Oxford handbook of construction grammar, ed. T. Hoffmann and G. Trousdale, 15–31. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., and D. Brentari. 2017. Gesture, sign and language: The coming of age of sign language and gesture studies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences: 1–82. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15001247.

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., A. Shield, D. Lenzen, M. Herzig, and C.A. Padden. 2012. The gestures ASL signers use tell us when they are ready to learn math. Cognition 123 (3): 448–453. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hay, J., and R. Baayen. 2005. Shifting paradigms: gradient structure in morphology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (7): 342–348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Himmelmann, N.P. 2004. Lexicalization and grammaticalization: Opposite or orthogonal? In What makes grammaticalization? A look from its fringes and its components, ed. W. Bisang, N. Himmelmann, and B. Wiemer, 21–42. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hockett, C.F. 1967. The Yawelmani basic verb. Language 43 (1): 208. https://doi.org/10.2307/411395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann, T., and G. Trousdale. 2013. Construction Grammar: Introduction. In The Oxford handbook of construction grammar, ed. id, 1–14. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hohenhaus, P. 2005. Lexicalization and institutionalization. In Handbook of word-formation, ed. P. Štekauer and R. Lieber, 353–373. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/1-4020-3596-9_15.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. 1975. Morphological and semantic regularities in the lexicon. Language 51 (3): 639–671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janzen, T. 1995. The Polygrammaticalization of FINISH in ASL. Master’s thesis. University of Manitoba, Winnepeg. Manitoba.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, T., and L. Ferrara. 2012. Lexicalization in signed languages: When is an idiom not and idiom? Selected Papers from UK-CLA Meetings 1: 229–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, T., and A. Schembri. 1999. On defining lexeme in a signed language. Sign Language & Linguistics 2 (2): 115–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kay, P., and C.J. Fillmore. 1999. Grammatical constructions and linguistic generalizations: The what’s X doing Y? Construction. Language 75 (1): 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. 2008. Some reflections on the relationship between “gesture” and “sign”. Gesture 8: 348–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klima, E.S., and U. Bellugi. 1979. The Signs of Language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kok, K. 2016. The grammatical potential of co-speech gesture: A functional discourse grammar perspective. Functions of Language 23 (2): 149–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kok, K., and A. Cienki. 2014. Taking simulation semantics out of the laboratory: Towards an interactive and multimodal reappraisal of embodied language comprehension. Language and Cognition 9: 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2014.25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R.W. 1987. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar: Theoretical Prerequisites. Vol. 1. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2000. A Dynamic Usage-Based Model. In Usage-based models of language, ed. M. Barlow and S. Kemmer. Stanford: CSLI Publications, Center for the Study of Language and Information.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2008. Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lepic, R. 2015. Motivation in morphology: Lexical patterns in ASL and English. Dissertation. University of California, San Diego, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2016. Lexical blends and lexical patterns in English and in American Sign Language, 98–111. Presented at the Quo Vadis Morphology? Online proceedings of the tenth Mediterranean Morphology Meeting. http://xantho.lis.upatras.gr/pasithee/index.php/mmm/article/view/2728.

  • Lepic, Ryan, Carl Börstell, Gal Belsitzman, and Wendy Sandler. 2016. Taking meaning in hand: Iconic motivations in two-handed signs. Sign Language and Linguistics 19 (1): 37–81. https://doi.org/10.1075/sll.19.1.02lep.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liddell, S. 2003. Grammar, gesture, and meaning in American Sign Language. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Liddell, S.K., and R.E. Johnson. 1986. American Sign Language compound formation processes, lexicalization, and phonological remnants. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 4 (4): 445–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liddell, S.K., and R.E. Johnson. 1989. American sign language: The phonological base. Sign Language Studies 64: 195–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liddell, S., and M. Metzger. 1998. Gesture in sign language discourse. Journal of Pragmatics 30 (6): 657–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lillo-Martin, D. and R. Meier. 2011. On the linguistic status of ‘agreement’ in sign languages. Theoretical Linguistics, 37(3-4): 95–141. Retrieved 6 February 2018, from https://doi.org/10.1515/thli.2011.009.

  • Marghetis, T., and B. Bergen. 2015. Embodied meaning, inside and out: The coupling of gesture and mental simulation. In Body-language-communication, ed. C. Mueller, A. Cienki, E. Fricke, S.H. Ladewig, D. McNeill, and S. Tessendorf. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan H. 2016. The effect of modality on signal space in natural languages. In The evolution of language: Proceedings of the 11th international conference (EVOLANG11), ed. S.G. Roberts, C. Cuskley, L. McCrohon, L. Barceló-Coblijn, O. Fehér, and T. Verhoef. Available online: http://evolang.org/neworleans/papers/192.html.

  • Núñez, R.E., and E. Sweetser. 2006. With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science 30: 401–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Padden, C.A. 1998. The ASL Lexicon. Sign Language & Linguistics 1 (1): 39–60. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.rice.edu/10.1075/sll.1.1.04pad.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Padden, C.A., I. Meir, S.-O. Hwang, R. Lepic, S. Seegers, and T. Sampson. 2013. Patterned iconicity in sign language lexicons. Gesture 13 (3): 287–308. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.13.3.03pad.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. 1999. Words and rules: The ingredients of language. New York: Harper Perennial.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., and A. Prince. 1988. On language and connectionism; analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition 28: 73–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 1994. Regular and irregular morphology and the psychological status of rules of grammar. In The reality of linguistic rules, ed. S.D. Lima, R.L. Corrigan, and G.K. Iverson. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandler, W. 2009. Symbiotic symbolization by hand and mouth in sign language. Semiotica 2009 (174): 241–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sandler, W., and D. Lillo-Martin. 2006. Sign language and linguistic universals. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schembri, A. 2003. Rethinking “classifiers” in signed languages. In Perpectives on classifier constructions in sign languages, ed. K. Emmorey, 3–34. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schembri, A., C. Jones, and D. Burnham. 2005. Comparing action gestures and classifier verbs of motion: evidence from Australian Sign Language, Taiwan Sign Language, and nonsigners’ gestures without speech. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 10 (3): 272–290. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/eni029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, E., and Y. Delaporte. 2014. A historical and etymological dictionary of American Sign Language: The origin and evolution of more than 500 signs. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singleton, J.L., J.P. Morford, and S. Goldin-Meadow. 1993. Once is not enough: Standards of well-formedness in manual communication created over three different timespans. Language 69 (4): 683–715. https://doi.org/10.2307/416883.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stokoe, W.C. 1960. Sign language structure. Silver Spring: Linstok Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Supalla, T. 1982. Structure and acquisition of verbs of motion and location in American Sign Language (Dissertation), University of California, San Diego.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 1986. The classifier system of American Sign Language. In Noun Classification: Proceedings of a Symposium on Categorization and Noun Classification, 181–214. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Supalla, T., and P. Clark. 2014. Sign Language Archaeology. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vigliocco, G., P. Perniss, and D. Vinson. 2014. Language as a multimodal phenomenon: implications for language learning, processing and evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 369 (1651): 20130292. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogler, C., and D. Metaxas. 2001. A Framework for Recognizing the Simultaneous Aspects of American Sign Language. Computer Vision and Image Understanding 81 (3): 358–384. https://doi.org/10.1006/cviu.2000.0895.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilcox, S., and C. Occhino. 2016. Constructing signs: Place as a symbolic structure in signed languages. Cognitive Linguistics 27(3): 371–404. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2016-0003.

  • Wilkinson, E. 2013. A functional description of SELF in American Sign Language. Sign Language Studies 13 (4): 462–490. https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.2013.0015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zima, E. 2014. English multimodal motion constructions. A construction grammar perspective. Studies van de BKL – Travaux du CBL – Papers of the LSB, Volume 8. http://uahost.uantwerpen.be/linguist/SBKL/sbkl2013/Zim2013.pdf.

  • Zwaan, R.A., and C.J. Madden. 2005. Embodied sentence comprehension. In The grounding of cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking, ed. D. Pecher and R.A. Zwaan, 224–245. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Geert Booij, Onno Crasborn, Wendy Sandler, Lynn Hou, and Hope Morgan for their careful and very helpful comments on this chapter. We also wish to acknowledge the input and contributions of Satu Siltaloppi, Stephanie Johnston, and Brennan Terhune-Cotter, as well as the audience members in the “Constructions in Language” session at the 2017 meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, for their thoughtful questions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ryan Lepic .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Lepic, R., Occhino, C. (2018). A Construction Morphology Approach to Sign Language Analysis. In: Booij, G. (eds) The Construction of Words. Studies in Morphology, vol 4. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-74394-3_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-74394-3_6

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-74393-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-74394-3

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics